SUBJECT INDEX
======================================
Harvey Sacks
Lectures on Conversation
Vol. I & II
(1992)
======================================
Compiled by Gail Jefferson
{Edited by Gene Lerner}
Absence |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 7 |
(Non-absence) of a name |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 18-19 |
Of ceremonials |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 38 |
Of membership in a unit |
|
Lecture 8 |
p. 60 |
M.I.R. categories & a notion of ‘absence’ |
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 88 |
Located via ‘order of relevance’ “Let's talk about there being, for some collection of classes, an ' order of relevancy' with respect to categories. It seems that a set of circumstances can provide that order of relevancy for some membership class.” |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 97 |
Of greetings |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 10 |
p. 190 |
Of ‘getting caught’ |
S ’66 |
Lecture 2 (R) |
pp. 261-262 |
Of greetings |
Lecture 04b |
pp. 293-295 |
Via ‘rounds’ |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 308 |
Via ‘invariably relevant’ items |
|
Lecture 29 |
p. 464 |
Via a set of categories |
|
Appendix A |
pp. 493-495 |
Vis-à-vis children’s games |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 11 |
p. 575 |
“I don’t do X” |
F ’67 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 670-671 |
Via utterance pairs |
S ’68 |
May 8 |
pp. 782-783 |
People’s orientation to what they’re missing…via absence of something to say |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 35-36 |
Greetings vis-à-vis ‘noticeable absence’ |
Lecture 5 |
p. 62 |
Greetings vis-à-vis ‘noticeable absence’ |
|
Lecture 6 |
p. 67 |
Introductions can be ‘notably absent’ |
|
S ’70 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 267-268 |
Discriminativeness necessary in reporting ‘what I didn’t do’. |
W ’71 |
March 4 |
p. 315 |
“After 25 years” of what? |
S ’71 |
April 19 |
p. 364 |
Lifting ‘transition relevance’; on somebody’s completion nobody talks and talk is not ‘absent’ |
April 23 |
p. 368 |
Invitation to come over and ‘talk’ provides for an absence (at least ‘dinner’) |
Academia |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 29-30 |
|
Lecture 10 |
p. 83 |
||
Lecture 13 |
p. 105 |
||
S ’67 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 558 |
|
Lecture 12 |
pp. 582-583 |
||
F ’67 |
Introduction |
p. 620 |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 5 |
|
Lecture 2 |
p. 17 |
||
W ’69 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 112 |
|
W ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 187 |
|
S ’70 |
Lecture 7 |
p. 282 |
|
S ’71 |
April 2 |
pp. 335-337 |
|
F ’71 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 419 |
|
Lecture 15 |
p. 507 |
||
S ’72 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 549-550 |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 561 |
Re. prefaces as independent of what they precede. |
Accountables, Accounts, Explanations, etc. |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 4 |
Accountable actions |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 19-20 |
Accountable actions |
|
Lecture 3 |
pp. 21-23 pp. 22-23 pp. 23-25 |
Getting an account via correction-invitation device Classes of accounts A3N = “Account apparently appropriate, negativer” |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 32 p. 37 |
|
|
Lecture 10 |
pp. 72-77 |
Accountable actions |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 101 |
The problem of ‘giving an account for silence’ |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 121-124 |
Among “competing facts”, the one with an explanation did occur. |
|
|
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 180 |
|
Lecture 10 |
pp. 190-191 |
|
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 195 |
Explaining away an asserted rule |
|
Lecture 14 |
p. 208 |
|
|
|
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 337-340 |
“Stereotyped” statements as ‘explanations’ |
Lecture 12 |
pp. 355-356 |
An explanation is the explanation; layers of accounts |
|
Lecture 15 |
pp. 380-381 |
Correction-invitation: a ‘possibly good account’ |
|
Lecture 20 |
pp. 412-413 |
An explanation is the explanation |
|
Lecture 21 |
pp. 425-426 |
Explanation of another’s position via some membership category |
|
Lecture 27 |
p. 453 |
In therapy, routinely usable explanations can be “just about irrelevant” |
|
Lecture 28 |
pp. 457-459 |
‘Causally efficacious’ categories (usable to formulate explanations) |
|
Lecture 32 |
p. 480 |
‘How they got it’ explanation of imitation |
|
Lecture 33 |
p. 487 |
What would a scientific account look like? |
|
W ’67 |
March 9 |
pp. 538, 542 pp. 545-546 |
Why say that now? Accounted for by talk to raised topics Why say that? Why now? Accounted for by its having been understood. |
|
|||
S ’67 |
Lecture 13 |
pp. 588-589 |
Category-bound action & its category explain why something happened: they do that. |
|
|||
F ’67 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 700 |
Rejecting the fact via rejecting the explanation: UFOs, the Dobu |
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 94-95 |
“This place costs too much money” to explain hole in shoe. |
|
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 195 |
Public vs. private accounts of sadness at a tragedy |
|
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 263-266 |
Delicate relationship between problem and explanation (blind lady – Brad Crandall) |
|
|||
S ’72 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 530 |
Why say this now? Accounted for via adjacency pair sequencing |
Activity-Occupied Phenomena |
|||
Volume I |
|||
W ’67 |
March 9 |
p. 543 |
“I did” |
Adjacency, Adjacency Pairs |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 150 |
“Consecutive” utterances |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 43 |
“Adjacency relationship between utterances” |
Lecture 4 |
p. 47 |
Placement and “adjacency relationship” |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 62 |
“Adjacency placing”… “ ‘utterance pairs’ ” |
|
W ’70 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 189-190 |
‘Greetings’ as “adjacency pairs” |
S ’71 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 521-532 |
Adjacency pairs: Scope of operation |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 533-537 |
Distribution rule for adjacency pairs |
|
S ’72 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 554-560 |
The relating power of adjacency |
Affiliation (see also Agreement) |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 12 |
p. 101 |
Jokes as ‘unaffiliated’ |
F ’65 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 148 |
Re. “We” |
Lecture 11 |
pp. 193, 195 |
Re. A maxim or rule |
|
Lecture 14 |
p. 206 |
As ‘military man’ |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 16 |
p. 383 |
“My” in its ‘affiliative’ use |
Lecture 21 |
p. 421 |
“We’ve told you that” |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 16 |
pp. 605-606 |
“My” in its ‘affiliative’ use |
Aggregate data (see ‘Collection’) |
Agreement |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 147 |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 21 |
p. 426 |
|
Lecture 23 |
pp. 428, 430-434 |
||
F ’67 |
Lecture 13 |
p. 736 |
“ ‘Well’ signals disagreement” |
S ’68 |
April 14 |
pp. 770, 771 |
Of a stock of stories, you remember the one that ‘agreed with the one you were told. |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 30 |
Agreement via “a same story,” disagreement via “a story which has you in a different position” |
W ’70 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 199 |
Slaves requiredly agree with masters |
S ’70 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 252, 256 |
Claiming vs. proving agreement |
Alternatives |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64-S ’65 |
Lecture 14 |
p. 116 |
Alternative sets of categories available for any population of persons |
F ’65 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 149 |
‘Company’ as a ‘general alternative category’ |
Lecture 7 |
p. 171 |
‘Company’ |
|
Lecture 10 |
p. 192 |
Alternative strategies: getting away with it vs. tempting enforcers |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 205-206 pp. 214 ff |
Alternative categorical formulations Personal- Impersonal |
|
S ’66
|
Lecture 2 |
p. 252 |
Alternative categories |
Lecture 4a |
p. 282 |
Proper alternative sequences |
|
Lecture 4 |
p. 305 |
Serious alternatives / alternation classes |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 307 |
Alternative identifications |
|
Lecture 6 |
p. 313 |
Alternative identifications |
|
Lecture 7 |
p. 326 |
Alternative identifications |
|
Lecture 11 |
pp. 348-350 |
“Alternative meanings for a word” |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 354 pp. 358-359 |
“Systematic differences in sorts of machinery invoked [for] doing warnings” “Complaining and applauding stand in some strong alternation to each other” |
|
Lecture 17 |
p. 392 p. 394 |
Alternative names for the same car Scope of a set of alternatives |
|
Lecture 18
|
p. 396 |
Alternative names of cars; teenager / hotrodder |
|
Lecture 20 |
pp. 415-416 |
Alternative name of cars |
|
Lecture 24 |
pp. 435-440 |
Alternative measuring systems |
|
Lecture 25
|
pp. 441-442 |
‘Alternation category: ‘company’ |
|
Lecture 26 |
pp. 443-449 |
Dilemma: either alternative is ‘bad’ |
|
Lecture 27 |
pp. 454 |
Doctor / patient: stranger / stranger (Freud) |
|
Lecture 31
|
p. 474 |
Legal-illegal / counts-doesn’t count |
|
Lecture 33
|
p. 486 |
Macro / micro: Catholicism / Protestantism |
|
Appendix A |
pp. 496-497 pp. 501-502 p.504 |
Vowel / consonant in first words; simple alternating actions in first games Legal-illegal / counts-doesn’t count Fantasy-reality theory – practical efficacy |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 562 pp. 564-565 |
‘Doctor’ / ‘just a somebody’ Consequences of alternative answers |
F ’67 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 679 |
More alternativeness vs. oriented-to contrast-class |
Lecture 14
|
pp. 740-742 |
Alternative temporal references |
|
S ’68 |
April 17 |
p. 753 |
Characterizations of persons...are in principle selections from alternatives. |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 6 |
p. 73 |
Alternative formulations of a topic |
W ’69 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 98-99 pp. 114-115, 123 |
‘Save’ vs. ‘unsafe’ compliments / ‘female’ vs. ‘male’ Alternative sequences |
Lecture 7 |
p. 117 |
“Why did you do X?” vs. “Why the hell did you do X?” |
|
Lecture 8 |
pp. 133-136 |
Abstract vs. concrete talk |
|
W ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 183 |
‘Precise’ vs. ‘approximate’ Temporal references |
Lecture 4 |
p. 195 |
Public vs. private accounts of sadness at a tragedy |
|
W ’71 |
March 4 |
p. 309 |
Alternative formulations for expressing position on reported event (“he had to go” vs. “he said he had to go”) |
S ’71 |
April 30 |
pp. 378 |
Alternative name-types |
May 21 |
pp. 404-405 p. 407 |
Alternative formulations of a person (“he,” “Mr. Jones,” “her husband”) ‘Stranger’ as an alternative formulation |
|
F ’71 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 447-448 |
“…some identities, both of which are correct, can stand in an alternation relationship to each other.” |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 455-456 |
‘We were going to [do X] but [A&B], so we [did Y]?’ “Question: Why put I a rejected alternative?” Re. proposing ‘normalness and variance’ |
|
Lecture 14
|
pp. 500-502 |
Wanted vs. unwanted possible Christmas gifts “I didn’t want an [X], I want a [Y] or a [Z].” |
|
S ’72 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 538 |
“The oldest one in the class” does not have as its set of alternatives the “obvious semantic” set; i.e. positions of age. Rather, e.g., “Are you the only black executive at [X] company?” (Methodological consideration here) |
Lecture 3 |
p. 547 |
Alternative types of answers |
Ambiguity |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 165-167 p. 167 |
‘You’ as ‘systematically ambiguous’ in warnings |
Lecture 9 |
p. 186 |
Who is “the guy”? |
|
Appendix A |
p. 225 |
Stage of life? family? (“baby”) |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 239 |
Stage of life? family? (“baby”) |
Lecture 1 (R) |
p. 247 |
Stage of life? family? (“baby”) |
|
Lecture 8 |
p. 335 |
“We” |
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 349 |
‘You’ as ‘systematically ambiguous’ |
|
Lecture 19 |
pp. 406-407 |
‘Nice kid’ / ‘hotrodder’ |
|
Lecture 20 |
p. 415 |
‘Not many people get picked up’ |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 13 |
pp. 584-585 |
‘The baby cried’; the criminal confessed / the Catholic confessed. |
F ’67 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 671-672 |
Sequentialized ambiguity: serious or joke? |
Volume II |
|||
S ’71 |
April 26 |
pp. 373-374 |
‘You’ as ambiguous |
F ’71 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 431-435 |
Re. obscene puns |
Lecture 12 |
pp. 489-490 |
Re. sounds of sex |
Announcements |
|||
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 87-91 |
‘Anonymous’ Interaction |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64-S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 3, 5 |
Agency / client |
Lecture 8 |
p. 59 |
And usability of ‘normal’ |
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 92 |
Adequate for checking out a noticing |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 114, 116 |
Re. seeing what has happened in a story one has only heard part of. (“Didn’t you smack her one?”) |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 185 |
Re. “Didn’t you smack her one.” |
S ’66
|
Lecture 4b |
pp. 297-298 |
Re. organizing greetings & introduction |
Lecture 20 |
p. 414 |
How “I will be seen by somebody unknown to me personally…” |
|
Lecture 24 |
p. 438 |
Between drivers, re. “How do you drive?” |
|
Lecture 32 |
p. 481 |
Seeing someone ‘imitating’ |
|
Lecture 33 |
p. 485 |
General statement |
|
Appendix A |
p. 492 pp. 504-505 |
In children’s games Across cultures (games) |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 555, 556 |
Vis-à-vis “How are you?” |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 558, 559, 560 |
Vis-à-vis “How are you?” |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 580 |
Controlling impressions for whomsoever |
|
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 95-96 |
Taxes as anybody’s complaint |
W ’70 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 191 pp. 194-195 p. 197 |
Greetings routinely done by people who are otherwise unacquainted Making one’s mind available to anonymous persons who encounter you Caring what anonymous people think of you |
S ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 221 |
Pedestrian-driver communication |
Lecture 4 |
p. 248 |
You should tell your friends a story in the same way “anybody should tell it to anybody” |
|
Lecture 7 |
pp. 280-281 |
“The rules of conversation are designed for anonymous parties” |
|
Lecture 15 |
pp. 510-511 |
Utterly conventional characterization in telling of an “intimate” problem: “there’s a way in which the story is about nobody in particular.” |
Answers [See “Questions & Answers”] |
Appearance |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64-S ’65 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 20 |
Re. Job’s Problem |
Lecture 14 |
pp. 120-121 |
Raymond [One Boy’s Day] and Adam |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 7 |
p. 173 |
Being seen as, e.g., ‘a hotrodder’ |
S ’66 |
Lecture 13 |
pp. 363-366, 369 pp. 404-409 |
Useability of, to create a false appearance (Button-Button) ‘Appearance verbs’ |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 79-80 |
Arranging to be seen in a “characteristic appearance”
|
Argument |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 179-181 |
Argument → name-calling |
Lecture 14 |
p. 207 |
The ‘unclean hands’ argument, etc. |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 10 |
pp. 344-346 |
Position markers “I still say though…” |
S ’67 |
Lecture 17 |
p. 614 |
Position markers “I still say though…” |
F ’67 |
Lecture 10 |
pp. 707-708 |
Communist vs. Republican / ‘people like us’ vs. ‘people like us’. |
Lecture 13 |
pp. 736-737 |
Locational marker “I still say though…” |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 49 |
Violation → Argument |
S ’71 |
April 5 |
pp. 344-347 |
Argument re. whether New Pike is “depressing” or not. |
F ’71 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 433 |
It’s a characteristically known thing that talk on any topic can “end up in an argument” |
Assembling Activities |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 10-11 |
|
Lecture 4 |
pp. 27, 30 |
||
Lecture 11 |
p. 89 |
(and ‘decomposing’) |
|
Lecture 13 |
p. 112 |
(assembling potential description) |
|
Volume II |
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 169 |
Assessments |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 66-69 |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 14 |
p. 206 |
|
S ’68 |
May 29 |
p. 800 |
|
Volume II |
|||
S ’71 |
May 3 |
pp. 384-390 |
Person’s technique for assessing possible correctness of offered solutions to their problems. |
Azande Oracle |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64-S ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 34-35 |
Base Environment
|
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64-S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 8-9 |
|
Volume II |
|||
S ’71 |
April 30 |
p. 380 |
(Same phenomena as ‘base environment’ but not so referred to) |
Believing, Believability, Reliability |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64-S ’65 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 62-64 |
Cry Wolf, missiles, etc. |
Volume II |
|||
W ’71 |
March 4 |
pp. 309-310 |
Exhibiting the believability or not of another’s reported talk (e.g. “he had to go” vs. “he said he had to go”) |
F ’71 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 462 |
“He said he went to acting school with Kirk Douglas. And I believe him.” (Here, he pre-positions “Nice looking guy” - cf. e.g. W ’70 Lecture 7 pp. 274-276 Re. pre-positioned characterizations, etc.) |
Lecture 10 |
pp. 479, 481 |
‘Suspension of disbelief’ |
|
Lecture 16 |
pp. 516-517 |
“How do you believe your brain?” (cat dream) |
Biblical Materials, Issues |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64-S ’65 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 20 |
Job’s Problem |
Lecture 4 |
p. 28 |
Bible study as a model |
|
Lecture 14 |
p. 118 p. 121 |
Job’s Problem Adam |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 7 |
p. 171 |
Abraham the Hebrew |
S ’66 |
Lecture 18 |
pp. 396-397 |
Abraham the Hebrew |
Lecture 20 |
p. 412 |
Job’s Problem |
|
Lecture 23 |
pp. 430-431 |
Ten Commandments / Hittite vassalage agreements |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 51 |
“That it may go well with thee…” |
S ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 220 |
Lot’s Warning Re. Sodom & Gomorrah |
Biology |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64-S ’65 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 27 ff |
|
Lecture 12 |
pp. 98-9 |
Case-by-Case Procedure |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 62 |
(Dealing with ‘odd’ suicides) |
Lecture 10 |
p. 78 |
(Kids testing Class I [causal] rules / Class II [normative] rules) |
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 89 |
(“A stream of odd ones”) |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 11 |
pp. 196-197 |
(Dealing with possible suicides) |
Categories and Classes, Categorizing |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 40-48 p. 45 |
M.I.R. ‘Categorizing the categorizer’ |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 58-59 p. 60 p. 64 |
“Normal” M.I.R. And ‘reliability’ |
|
Lecture 9 |
pp. 68-71 |
M.I.R. ; ‘imitation’ |
|
Lecture 11 |
pp. 86-87 p. 88 |
M.I.R. “classes & their categories permit you to see” A categorizing B, gets a sense of C. (cf. Lecture 6 above, p. 45: ‘Categorizing the categorizer’ and Schegloff’s Introduction, p. xxxiv) |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 116-117 |
Inference = (among other things), categorizing an event one has not seen. |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 148-149 p. 149 |
‘We’ can refer to a category ‘Company’ as a ‘general alternation category’ |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 170-172 |
‘Company’ |
|
Lecture 8 |
pp. 179-181 |
Category-bound activities |
|
Lecture 9 |
pp. 183-185 p. 186 |
‘Character appears on cue’ Category-bound activities |
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 195 |
Category assigned to a position being esposed |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 201 |
‘Returning spirits…’ as a category for handling strangers |
|
Lecture 14 |
p. 205 ff |
Alternative categorical formulation |
|
Appendix A |
p. 225 ff p. 228 |
Categorical use of “the mommy” |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 238 ff pp. 241-242 |
‘Membership categorization device’ Category-bound activities |
Lecture 1 (R) |
p. 246 ff pp. 248-250 |
Membership categorization device Category-bound activities |
|
Lecture 2 |
pp. 252-254 |
Category-bound activities |
|
Lecture 2 (R) |
pp. 259-261 |
Category-bound activities |
|
Lecture 04.b |
pp. 298-299 |
Locating appropriate set of categories |
|
Lecture 4 |
pp. 301-302 |
Category-bound activities / identification |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 306 |
Vis-à-vis identification |
|
Lecture 6 |
p. 313 pp. 314-315 pp. 317-319 |
Vis-à-vis identification Vis-à-vis omni-relevant devices Vis-à-vis ‘cover identifications’ |
|
Lecture 7 |
pp. 326-327 |
Vis-à-vis identification; relational pair |
|
Lecture 8 |
pp. 334-340 pp. 336-340 |
‘Membership categorization device” Category-bound activities |
|
Lecture 10 |
pp. 346-347 |
‘Positioned’ categories |
|
Lecture 13 |
pp. 367-368 |
Categories aren’t persons; ‘family’ device |
|
Lecture 18 |
pp. 396-403 |
‘Hotrodders’ as a revolutionary category |
|
Lecture 19 |
pp. 406-407 pp. 408-409 |
‘Nice kid’ If you look like someone who does X, then what you’re doing is X. |
|
Lecture 21 |
pp. 417-418 pp. 425-426 |
Vis-à-vis identification Position in an argument as category-bound |
|
Lecture 25 |
pp. 441-442 |
Alternative category: ‘company’ |
|
Lecture 26 |
pp. 444-449 |
Cops, teachers, etc.: “a special class of people” vis-à-vis teenagers; for “giving lip back” |
|
Lecture 27 |
p. 454-455 |
Doctor / patient vs. stranger / stranger (in Freud) |
|
Lecture 28 |
p. 457 p. 459 |
‘Causally efficacious’ categories (usable to formulate explanations) Re. social sciences using Members’ devices to formulate its accounts (cf. Lecture 04b. pp. 295, 299) |
|
Lecture 29 |
pp. 464-466 |
Male-female (safe compliments) |
|
Lecture 30 |
pp. 467-469 |
Selection problem for social scientists |
|
Lecture 32 |
pp. 480-481 |
Inverse to category-bound activities (imitation). |
|
Appendix A |
pp. 490-499 pp. 502-503 |
Members categorization devices in children’s games Categories in children’s games |
|
W ’67 |
February 16 |
p. 515 |
Omni-relevant device, patient-therapist |
March 2 |
p. 532 |
Invokability of ‘patient-therapist’ |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 550 p. 552 p. 553 pp. 555-556 |
Category-bound ‘correctness’ Class: ‘proper conversationalists’ Class: ‘non-proper conversationalists’ Classes: Personal States and Value States |
Lecture 9 |
p. 562 |
‘Doctor’ vs. ‘just a somebody’ |
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 577 |
‘Stereotypes’ as valuable |
|
Lecture 12 |
pp. 582-583 |
Critique of ‘category-bound activities’ |
|
Lecture 13 |
pp. 584-588 pp. 585-587 pp. 588-589 |
Proving that something is a ‘category-bound activity’ ‘Positioned’ categories An important use of ‘category-bound activities’ |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 590-594 pp. 594-596 |
Category ‘partitioning’ ‘Omni-relevant’ category collection |
|
Lecture 15.1 |
pp. 597-600 |
Category ‘partitioning’ |
|
Lecture 15.2 |
p. 601 |
‘Category-bound’ topic: automobile discussion |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 630 |
Vis-à-vis expressing an opinion |
Lecture 011 |
pp. 712-713 pp. 713-714 |
Focusing on a relevant identity category via “he” Categorical vs. personal reference via “we” / “they” |
|
S ’68 |
April 17 |
p. 757 |
Attention to ‘topic’ via co-class membership (e.g. cigars and pipes) |
May 29 |
p. 797 |
‘Negro woman’ vs. ‘employee’ |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 52 |
Interruption as a case of the class rudeness |
Lecture 6 |
p. 78 |
“Characteristic” way of doing ‘member’ talk |
|
W ’69 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 99 pp. 101-102 p. 103 |
Set of people by reference to ‘male-female’ Vis-à-vis determining what an action done by A to B is doing to C. Partitioned categories |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 104-111 p. 110 p. 112 |
‘Patients / observers’ as ‘performers / audience’ Partitioned categories Producing “a correct sequence of actions for some set [of categorized terms].” |
|
Lecture 7 |
pp. 120-121 |
“Higher ranked person can correct.” |
|
W ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 182 |
“‘They’ is in principle a pronoun not for plurals but for categories…” |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 194-197 |
Stranger-stranger interaction during disaster / tragedy |
|
S ’70 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 250 |
A class: “1st Stories”; a class: “2nd Stories” |
W ’71 |
February 19 |
pp. 296-298 |
Host / guest vis-à-vis complainables |
S ’71 |
April 19 |
pp. 360-366 |
Caller / Called |
May 21 |
pp. 405-407 |
Service personnel doing a job vs. “stranger” being “bothered” |
|
F ’71 |
Lecture 10 |
p. 482 |
A “crucially ‘mother-daughter’ interaction” |
Lecture 11 |
p. 490 |
The ‘mother-daughter’ joke |
|
Lecture 15 |
p. 510 |
A ‘rational’ ‘father’ and ‘emotional’ ‘mother’: Adequate characterizations (Verges in categorical-bound activities.) |
|
S ’72 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 538 |
“The oldest one in the class” as a ‘unique position’, akin to “the only (cop, Negro, woman) in the class.” |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 542-553 p. 544 |
‘Caller-Answer-Called’ etc. Pone-call ‘answerer’, etc. as “categories and not merely the person they are” |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 569 |
“Whole classes of types of relationships in social structural terms, employers / employees, etc., are characterized by admitting beginnings [of conversation] and no more; specifically not admitting transforms of beginnings into first topics.” |
Causality / Chance |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 14 |
‘Causally bound’ relationships |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 34-35 p. 36 |
Azande vs. us. Private calendars: ‘causally powerful’ |
|
Lecture 10 |
pp. 77-78 |
Children and Class I, II rules |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 124-125 |
A phenomenon? Or statistical chance? (Miracles, psychic research) |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 28 |
p. 457 |
‘Causally efficacious’ categories |
Ceremonials |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 14-19 pp. 15-16 |
“How are you?” “Fine”, etc. Jokes, games, performances |
Lecture 7 |
p. 55 |
Re. “holding the floor” |
|
Volume II |
|||
S ’71 |
April 26 |
pp. 370-372, 374 |
‘An evening together’ as “a ceremony of some sort.” |
Children |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 70 |
(and Negroes: imitation) |
Lecture 10 |
pp. 76-77 pp. 78-79 |
(and infantile adults: “X told me to”) (and Class I and II rule) |
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 84 |
(Unentitled wisdom as ‘error’) |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 98 |
(and a ‘minimal conversation’) |
|
Lecture 13 |
pp. 111-112 |
(Learning ‘potential description’) |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 114-115 p. 120 |
(First lie) and subversions |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 155-156 |
Children’s speech (‘buildups’) |
Lecture 6 |
p. 167 |
and Warnings (see Class I, II Rules, pp. 78-79) |
|
Lecture 7 |
pp. 172-174 |
‘Teenager’ vs. ‘hot rodder’ |
|
Appendix A |
p. 226 pp. 227-228 |
Use of “it”: pronouns available Use of “the mommy”: family device |
|
Appendix B |
p. 230 |
Restricted rights to talk → “You know what?” |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 255-258 pp. 256-258 |
“Competence to lie” / building stories Restricted rights to talk |
Lecture 2 (R) |
pp. 263-265 |
Restricted rights to talk |
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 348 |
‘Build-ups’ |
|
Lecture 13 |
pp. 363-369 p. 368 |
Button-Button Use of expanded ‘family’ device |
|
Lecture 16 |
pp. 386-387 |
Kid recognizing a ‘possessitive’ |
|
Lecture 18 |
pp. 398-399 |
Children’s culture |
|
Lecture 30 |
p. 472 |
Children’s vocabularies |
|
Lecture 31 |
pp. 473-478 p. 474 |
Games: legal / illegal Children’s memory for correct speech |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 565 |
When children begin systematically to lie |
Lecture 13 |
p. 588 |
Kids learn ‘programmatic relevance’ early |
|
Lecture 14 |
p. 591 |
Preparing for ‘adult roles’ |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 629 |
Two children of the British autocracy |
Lecture 7 |
p. 680 |
Kids’ play with language involves specific attention to ‘the formal features of a language’. |
|
Lecture 8 |
p. 689 |
Children “rather well attuned to the pre-sequence phenomena.” |
|
Lecture 14 |
p. 739 |
Young child doing paraphrasing. |
|
S ’68 |
April 17 |
p. 760 |
Children have strong place-memory |
Volume II |
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 12 |
pp. 489-490 pp. 490-492 |
Children’s puzzlement re. sounds of sex Children’s encounters with scope of application of rules. |
Claiming vs. Demonstrating |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 146-147 |
|
Volume II |
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 252, 260 |
|
F ’71 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 436 |
Closings |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 12 |
p. 96 |
No generic way to close a conversation |
F ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 160-161 |
Closing an insult sequence (“Face the music”) |
S ’66 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 255-256 |
A story close / built as an ending |
Lecture 2 (R) |
p. 263 |
A story close / proper ending |
|
Lecture 21 |
pp. 420-421 |
Withdrawing from an insult sequence (“Face the Music”) |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 678-679 |
Closing an insult sequence with “Thank you” |
S ’68 |
May 8 |
p. 778 |
Closing a call with reintroduced reason for call / Relationship between beginnings and closings. |
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 88 |
Closing is collaboratively done and takes a sequence |
S ’71 |
April 19 |
pp. 363-366 |
“It’s caller’s business to find a place to stick in a possible closing.” |
May 21 |
pp. 402-403 |
SPC:NYE call closing |
|
S ’72 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 552 |
“Calleds are ‘forced’ off the phone: ‘I’ve got to go do something,’ Callers ‘offer’ to get off the phone: ‘I’m holding up your line’…that…suggests that it is Caller’s business to get off the phone first, Calleds doing it in extremis.” |
Coincidence |
|||
Volume II |
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 238-240 |
|
F ’71 |
Lecture 10 |
pp. 478-479 |
Collaborative Utterances |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 144-147 |
|
Lecture 6 |
p. 167 |
||
S ’66 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 321-323 |
|
Lecture 15 |
p. 379 |
||
Lecture 21 |
p. 421 |
||
W ’67 |
March 2 |
pp. 528-529 |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 651-655 |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 57-60 |
|
Lecture 6 |
pp. 71, 82-83 |
Collections: Working with, Arguing via, etc. |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 3 p. 9 p. 11 |
“I have a large collection of these conversations” “In my materials, again and again [X occurs].” “We’re talking about objects that can be found elsewhere |
Lecture 2 |
p. 13 p. 14 p. 15 p. 16 p. 17 p. 19 p. 20 |
“Recurrently in these conversations [X occurs]. And…I have things from police reports where [X occurs]. “Persons who [do X] talk about themselves routinely as [Y].” “Routinely if you look at [X conversations] they begin like [Y]. “I came across a very frequently recurring kind of statement.” “A recurrent thing I’ve seen throughout their stuff is…” “One of the things reported about persons who [do X] is [Y]. “Most people, when [X occurs], will say [Y].” |
|
Lecture 3 |
p. 23 p. 25 |
“These things are not only recurrent, but…they do work...” “Again and again we find that [X occurs].” |
|
Lecture 4 |
p. 29 |
[The more material you have at your command, the more you ought to be able to…see…recurrence. But the way to proceed is item by item.” |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 36 |
“I came across [X] several times in the telephone conversation I’ve been analyzing.” |
|
Lecture 6 |
p. 45 |
“There is…a whole large bunch of [X statements].” |
|
Lecture 7 |
p. 49 |
60 first lines of ‘pickups’; “more than 50 were questions.” |
|
Lecture 8 |
p. 57 p. 64 |
“Recurrently in these conversations [X occurs]” “Maybe it just happened that time. Not so. Recurrently [X occurs].” |
|
Lecture 9 |
p. 66 |
“Suicidal persons recurrently say [X].” |
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 94 |
(Re. “basic assumption, which could have been wrong”: “But…I know that people can do this, I’ve watched it many times...[But] it could have been the case that everybody came back and said, ‘No, I never saw that happen.’ And that’s possible. It might be something that’s dying out. A thing our forefathers had. Like God.” |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 102 |
“I got 60 first lines [of ‘pickups’], of which just under 60 were questions. What I had wanted to be saying [to the class], and which they could see once they had these collections, was…” |
|
Lecture 13 |
p. 105 |
“Homans’ procedure for starting a book is one of the most recurrent you’ll find” |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 121-124 |
4 cases of a procedure: “It’s absolutely routinely used.” |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 6 |
p. 166 |
(Re. “you”) “I have a lot of very subtle usages.” “Those kinds of uses are recurrent” |
Lecture 7 |
p. 174 |
(Re. not recognizing moves of a non-member) “That kind of thing is very regularly done for all sorts of other things. “[Volkswagons don’t get waved at].” |
|
Lecture 10 |
p. 192 |
“…in very different materials on finds the same [phenomenon].” |
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 195 |
“We can get lots of examples of [X].” |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 2 (R) |
p. 263 |
“Kids around the age of three [do X].” |
Lecture 04.a |
pp. 283-284 |
‘Rule detection’ via collection vs. single case |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 306 |
For some assertion, “provide alternative’ independent materials…” |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 354 p. 355 p. 359 |
[X] can be “dug out of such materials as we’ve got (but I take it you’d need a considerable amount to do it)…” [X] would involve some larger corpus of materials, since [it’s] basically a statistical argument “…some people do [X] with immense regularity.” |
|
Lecture 21 |
p. 417 |
“…we want to ask…whether such activities…occur recurrently in the order they occur in here” |
|
Lecture 27 |
p. 455 |
“I take it that [X] is quite formulatable and probably very recurrent.” |
|
Lecture 31 |
p. 473 |
“And here is another, similar piece of data…” |
|
W ’67 |
March 2 |
p. 533 |
“It’s all over the place”…”It’s seeing [various features] and then when you look back to it seeing that it’s all over the place, that I then understand, Alright now, attention has to be paid…[to X]. |
March 9 |
p. 540 p. 545 |
“We would have to check out whether such things are in fact done, so that we could see [that a move in this fragment is indeed] not legitimate, conventional, preferred, etc.” “Indirect reference” as “the most usual ways to talk.” |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 549 |
“In some tape I had, I came across a statement that I’d heard before…” |
F ’67 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 651 |
Re. collaborative completions: “[I first thought] it must be quite rare, even though that they do it at all is an extremely important fact. But it turns out to be an extremely frequent and routinely doable thing.” |
Lecture 5 |
p. 662 |
“It is typically, routinely, overwhelmingly the case that…” |
|
Lecture 7 |
p. 682 |
Floor seekers are “terribly widely used.” |
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 723 |
“A very characteristic way to [do X] is [Y]. Here’s some cases.” (3 cases shown)
|
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 725 |
“I won’t quote cases of that sort, but I take it that one can see that it’s so.” |
|
S ’68 |
May 29 |
p. 798 |
Re. letters sent to Governor Brown during Chessman case. “Something like 40,000 letters, and I’ve read several hundred of them…you get one standard pattern…” |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 5 p. 6 p. 11 p. 13 p. 14 |
Re. 2nd stories’ intense relationship to 1sts : “What we need to do…is to watch conversations hereafter and see.” Re. work of 2nd story: “So, for example, in other materials [X occurs].” “[X] is not at all common, usual, done” “[X] is an enormously common thing, and to get a sense of it as a usual piece of rhetoric, here’s a yesterday-instance of it.” “And anybody knows that when somebody [does X they’re telling you [Y].”
|
Lecture 2 |
pp. 19-20 p. 20 p. 25 p. 25 |
“If it’s not too easy to see the point with one case, then with another case you might be able to get a glimpse of a kind of parallel thing going on.” “If you look at stories comparatively…” “Answers are, with some recurrence, not sentences.” “One might figure that [X is] incidental, so I’ll read another. If you get two you might feel a little better. Or you might figure it’s still just chance.” |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 59 |
“When I first found [X] I was absolutely awed…And then [I thought] ‘Does it ever happen?’ And we searched around and found it’s really extremely common” |
|
W ’69 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 138 p. 143 |
“To make [X] argument you need…to have different cases so as to be able to see what is being argued. That’s why I picked two cases…and indeed it was noticing the two cases that set the whole thing up.” “[X] is the sort of phenomenon that, given one instance you might figure it’s a very rare case, but you could collect a bunch of them in a week.” |
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 168-169 |
“To come to [X notion], we need…a series of other calls in which we could say, establish ‘normal [Y]’…” |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 175-176 |
“[X phenomena] require a consideration of some collection of [Y’s]…” |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 208 |
“And if we find it hard to empathize with the particulars of this conversation, we might imagine other sorts of things…” |
|
S ’70 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 230 |
Having X phenomenon, “one could then collect various other ways that [such things] are done.” |
W ’71 |
February 19 |
p. 292 |
“I find an awful lot of [X phenomenon]. And usually, once I find an awful lot of something I wonder if there isn’t something to it, and begin to develop an account of how it would happen. Finding one, you could just say, Well, I’m being artful in finding it…” |
F ’71 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 422-423 |
Recommendation: “Take an object like proverbial expressions. Subject them to a distributional investigation to see what’s done with them.” |
Lecture 2 |
p. 429 p. 430 |
“A common recurrence in a word misuse is [X]…” It happens that I have lots of materials involving these people and it’s a very common thing for [X and Y to occur] It doesn’t particularly add all that much to these materials that that’s so…” “A typical kind of incompletion which won’t get completed by someone else: where you’re…going to report on something obscene.” |
|
Lecture 10 |
p. 481 |
“Routinely the materials used in a punch line involve [X]…” |
|
Lecture 16 |
p. 512 |
“I saw this [datum] and began to puzzle about it by virtue of having had such an experience…it occurred to me that there was good reason to think that it might be an instance of the same thing.” |
|
S ’72 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 530 |
“Characteristically stories begin with something that we call a ‘story preface’…[which is] typically a sentence [like X]…” |
Lecture 3 |
p. 543 p. 547 |
“If you look at [X event], it turns out that this [Y occurrence] is not at all odd.” “We would want to do an investigation as to whether [X} occurs…with some interesting frequency.” |
Commitment |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 5 |
To an account |
Lecture 3 |
p. 25 |
To correctness of proverbs |
|
Lecture 9 |
p. 66 |
To what the society holds is important or sacred |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 7 |
p. 174 |
To being a hotrodder |
Lecture 10 |
pp. 193, 194 |
To rule-makers, enforcers, etc. |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 29 |
pp. 465 |
To some position / to “the special characteristics of this girl” |
Volume II |
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 186-187 |
To a trust in ‘vision’…what one “simply sees”. |
S ’70 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 259 |
To the proper operation of your memory in conversation time |
S ’71 |
April 30 |
p. 380 p. 380 |
By suicidal callers, to “this world” Asking suicidal callers to give their name is asking for a problematic sort of commitment. |
F ’71 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 420 |
To the use of observation as a basis for theorizing |
Lecture 6 |
p. 457 |
To “the normal preferences” |
Comparison / Equivalence |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 23 |
p. 431 |
|
Lecture 24 |
p. 439 |
||
Lecture 28 |
pp. 456-460 |
||
Lecture 29 |
p. 464 |
||
W ’67 |
February 16 |
p. 522 |
(Equivalence of settings) |
S ’67 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 560 |
Terms within each subsets of [+] [0] [-] are treated as equivalent (Re. responding to HAY) |
F ’67 |
Lecture 14 |
pp. 740-743 |
(Non-equivalent ‘identicals’: “Tuesday” vs. “November 11, 1967”) |
S ’68 |
May 29 |
pp. 789-790 |
(“X is just like Y”) |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 6 |
p. 77 |
Constraints on to whom somebody can be compared |
S ’72 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 568-569 |
Second speakers for talk about (e.g. disasters as events in “our lives”) talk about them comparatively (1st speaker says X happened, 2nd says “to me too” or “not to me”, etc.), “Though the events didn’t happen comparatively.” |
Complaints |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 12 |
pp. 358, 359-360 |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 549-550 |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 11 |
pp. 575-577 |
|
Lecture 15.1 |
pp. 599-600 |
‘Safe’ complaints |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 634-638 |
About violations of ‘one at a time’ |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 45-55 |
“You interrupted me” |
F ’69 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 94-97 |
“This place costs too much money.” |
Fragment |
pp. 150-151 |
“You never take me anywhere” |
|
W ’71 |
February 19 |
pp. 296-298 |
“Wasn’t that the dirtiest place?” |
March 4 |
pp. 313-314 p. 316 |
A ‘fragile’ complaint (Louise’s Friday night) Building a reasonable complaint |
|
F ’71 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 433 |
Having made a complaint, the complaining itself becomes the topic. |
Lecture 14 |
pp. 502-503 |
“That guy” in a complaint by wife about husband |
Completion |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 12 |
pp. 96, 103 |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 158-159 |
‘Incomplete utterances’ |
S ’66 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 310-311 |
‘Adequate complete utterances’; ‘utterance completers’ |
Lecture 15 |
pp. 376-377 |
Indicating that a sentence is not a complete utterance |
|
W ’67 |
March 2 |
pp. 524-526, 527 |
Orientation to completion |
F ’67 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 643-645 |
Orientation to completion / complete and incomplete utterances. “transition events” |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 647-655 |
Utterance completion |
|
Lecture 5 |
pp. 656-659 |
Utterance completion / complete actions |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 33-34
pp. 39-40 p. 43 |
‘Completion-transition points’
Recognizably complete utterances ‘Adequate complete utterance’ |
Lecture 4 |
p. 44 |
“First ‘possible completion’ to be treated as actual completion |
|
Lecture 5 |
pp. 57-58 pp. 59,60 |
Listening for ‘completion’ “Possible utterance ends” |
|
W ’69 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 109 |
Seeable completion of a one-word utterance |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 144-146 |
Signaling utterance completion / incompletion |
|
W ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 182 |
Signaling utterance completion / incompletion |
Lecture 4 |
p. 191 |
A greeting as an ‘adequate complete utterance’ |
|
S ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 224 p. 228 |
“Next possible completion” Story completion |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 286-287 |
Packaging one’s opinions into a story ‘what she said, what I said’, etc. giving no chance for quarrel by current coparticipants “at each sentence-end.” |
|
F ’71 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 430 |
Incompletion vis-à-vis obscenity |
Lecture 4 |
p. 438 |
Completing another’s utterance |
|
S ’72 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 527 |
‘Possible completion’ via adjacency pairs |
Compliments |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 60-61 |
‘Safe compliment’ |
S ’66 |
Lecture 29 |
pp. 464-465 |
‘Weak, safe compliment’ |
S ’67 |
Lecture 15.1 |
pp. 597-599 |
‘Safe’ compliments |
S ’68 |
May 29 |
pp. 793-794 |
Compliments produced as ‘indirect quotes’ (“I just wanted to say how much I enjoyed your class this morning”) |
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 98-103 |
‘Safe compliments’ |
S ’70 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 272-273 p. 278 |
“Boy there goes a great gal” ‘Safe compliments’ |
W ’71 |
February 19 |
p. 296 |
Complimenting person who brought you to a restaurant, instead of the chef |
Context |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 14 |
pp. 370-375 |
|
Lecture 27 |
pp. 450-455 |
Contrast |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 11 |
p. 88 |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 12 |
p. 358 |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 679-680 |
Vis-à-vis sarcasm |
Lecture 13 |
p. 736 |
Via emphasis: “No let’s take my car.” |
|
S ’68 |
May 29 |
pp. 787-788 pp. 798-800 |
Contrast class ‘true-false’ In-and-out |
Volume II |
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 171-172 |
‘Today’ as one among a contrast class: ‘time references’ vs. unique applicability |
W ’71 |
March 4 |
pp. 315-316 |
Finding a ‘deleted’ term via occurrence of its contrast: “After 25 years [of X] they got divorced.” X=marriage. |
S ’71 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 558 |
Contrast-stressing intonation |
Conveying Information (See also ‘Getting Something Done…”) |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 04.a |
p. 283 |
Via order of introductions |
Correction-Invitation Device |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 21-23 |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 15 |
pp. 380-381 |
|
|
Correctness / Truth |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 13 |
pp. 105, 110-111 |
Proverbs as “correct for something” |
Lecture 14 |
pp. 114, 116 p. 118 p. 118-119 |
Of a guess ‘Presumptive correctness’ of an application of a procedure (Job’s Problem) Presumptively correct descriptions |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 114, 116 p. 118 p. 118-119 |
Of a guess ‘Presumptive correctness’ of a application of a procedure (Job’s Problem) Presumptively correct descriptions |
|
|
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 14 |
p. 206 |
Incidentally correct for [X] |
Lecture 6 |
p. 166 |
Proverbially correct |
|
Lecture 12
|
pp. 358, 360 |
Of some possible fact |
|
|
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 21 |
pp. 424-425 |
Of “if we start out discussing, we end up fighting” |
Lecture 26 |
p. 444 |
Of a statement |
|
Lecture 30 |
p. 469 |
Of something the natives say. |
|
Lecture 31 |
pp. 474-475 |
Children’s monitoring of correctness in speech |
|
Appendix A |
p. 501 p. 506 |
Young children’s talk Children’s correcting of errors in speech |
|
Appendix A |
p. 501 p. 506 |
Young children’s talk Children’s correcting of errors in speech |
|
|
|||
S ’67 |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 557-566 p. 564 |
Truth of a Member’s statement (“Everyone Has to Lie”) Answers constructed by reference to a procedure’s use and not by reference to what is ‘correct’ |
Lecture 12 |
p. 581 |
‘Correctly’ produced hints; ‘correctly’ need not mean ‘true’. (vis-à-vis ‘phoney’) |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 740-742 |
Of alternative ‘correct’ temporal references, one can be appropriate, another crazy. |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 740-742 |
Of alternative ‘correct’ temporal references, one can be appropriate, another crazy. |
|
|
|||
S ’68 |
May 29 |
p. 787 |
‘Showing an intention of truth by some statement.’…‘exhibiting “correctness”.’ |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 74, 80 |
“Because it’s true” is irrelevant as an account. |
|
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 115-116 |
‘Password’ answers as ‘correct’ in some independence of being true. |
|
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 222 |
Correctness vs. relevance |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 234-236 |
Assessable correctness of a story |
|
|
|||
W ’71 |
March 4 |
pp. 316-317 |
Possible correctness of a members’ formulation (“He was being very rational”) |
|
|||
S ’71 |
May 3 |
pp. 384-390 |
Persons’ techniques for assessing possible correctness of an offered solution to their problem. |
Lecture 4 |
p. 443 |
Listening technique for spouses: monitoring the other’s stories for its correct presentation; putting in corrections. |
|
|
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 447 |
One “correct” identity replaced by another instead of cumulated. |
Counting |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 60-63 |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 14 |
p. 208 |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 305 |
|
Lecture 12 |
pp. 360-362 |
||
Lecture 24 |
pp. 435-440 |
Measurement Systems |
Couples-talk |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’67 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 690 |
Vis-à-vis pre-invitations |
Lecture 10 |
p. 703-704 |
Vis-à-vis separation of sexes at gatherings |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 437-443 |
How to listen to spouse’s retold stories |
Lecture 14 |
pp. 502-503 |
“That guy” in complaint by wife about husband |
|
Lecture 15 |
pp. 506-507 |
‘Wives’ perhaps not in full agreement with a story version (Kim: “We should have just left everything alone” and Jan: “He probably knew”) |
Description |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 30-31 |
|
Lecture 6 |
p. 42 |
||
Lecture 8 |
p. 65 |
||
Lecture 13 |
pp. 110-112 |
‘Potential descriptions’: Proverbs |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 12 |
p. 201 |
‘Potentially correct description’ |
Appendix A |
p. 228-229 |
‘Potentially correct description’ |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 237 |
‘Possible description’ (baby cried…) |
Lecture 1 (R) |
p. 245 ff |
‘Possible description’ (baby cried…) |
|
Lecture 2 |
p. 252 ff |
‘Possible description’ (baby cried…) |
|
Lecture 30 |
pp. 469-471 |
Describability |
|
Appendix A |
pp. 498-500 |
Describability of children’s games |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 13 |
p. 588 |
Identification via category bound action as ‘correct description’ |
S ’68 |
May 29 |
p. 795 |
Describing events via that they happened in teller’s life. |
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 147-149 |
Preference for ‘you know what I’m talking about’ description |
Fragment |
pp. 150-151 |
Alternative descriptive verbs; e.g., ‘helping X’ vs. ‘doing’; ‘taking X to’ vs. going to’. |
|
W ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 180-181 |
“Recognition-type descriptions” |
S ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 215-221 |
Descriptions built to be ‘ordinary’ |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 231-232, 236 pp. 234-236 |
“Course-of-action” report, into which a story is placed. Assessable correctness of a description |
|
S ’72 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 540-541 |
Descriptions vary across to whom the thing is being described |
Directedness of an Utterance |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 14 |
p. 219 |
News reporter’s question on behalf of public |
S ’66 |
Lecture 04.b |
pp. 296-299 |
Instructions directed to A, hold for B, C, D |
Lecture 15 |
pp. 377-378 |
1st speaker rule regulates speaker and non-speakers |
|
Lecture 16 |
p. 388 |
Possessive pronoun use shifts via who’s being talked to. |
|
W ’67 |
March 2 |
pp. 529-534 |
Instructions directed to A, hold for B, C, D. Doing X to A is doing Y to B. |
S ’67 |
Lecture 17 |
p. 610 |
Possessive pronoun use varies by reference to who is talking to whom. |
F ’67 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 665-666 |
Instructions directed to A, hold for B, C, D. Doing X to A is doing Y to B. |
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 91 p. 93 |
Noticing directed to the person who was just talking ‘Conversational action’: saying X about Y’s Z can be ‘flirting’, ‘insulting’, etc. |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 99-102 |
Doing X to A is doing Y to B |
|
W ’70 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 191-192 |
Greetings directed to “somebody”; to whom can be “a technical problem”. |
S ’70 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 276-281 |
What does a non-“direct recipient” of an utterance make of it? ; Doing X to A is doing Y to B. |
S ’72 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 553 |
Telephone answerer as ‘overhearing’ called’s feelings about called, exhibited in how caller asks answerer for called. |
Dreams |
|||
Volume II |
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 16 |
pp. 512-518 |
Eating Together |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’68 |
May 29 |
pp. 791-792, 794 |
|
Volume II |
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 203-204 |
|
W ’71 |
March 11 |
pp. 318-331 |
(‘Herring’) |
Emblems |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 17 |
pp. 392-395 |
|
Volume II |
|||
S ’67 |
Lecture 17 |
p. 612 |
Entitlement |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 68-69
|
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 91 |
||
F ’65 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 180 |
|
Volume II |
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 243-248 |
Entitlement to experience |
Erasability |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 61-62 |
|
Lecture 13 |
p. 108 |
Error |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 11 |
p. 84 |
Experts’ errors |
F ’65 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 186 |
Grammatical awkwardness (“the guy’ll pick the guy up”) provides occasion for emphasis (the repeat of “dirty grubby tee shirt”) |
S ’66 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 337 |
No occasions for seeing ‘incorrect’ness |
Lecture 31 |
pp. 474-475 |
||
Appendix A |
p. 506 |
Detectable errors |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 641-642 |
Grammatical awkwardness provides for unplannedness of a rebuke. (“I thought I could help him with supervision”) |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 690-691 |
A request for information ‘corrected to’ a pre-request (“What is – what are those, cigars?”) |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 724 |
Names: predictably wrongly transcribed |
|
Lecture 13 |
pp. 730-735 |
Tying based mis-hearings |
|
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 143 |
‘Gist preserving errors’ |
W ’71 |
March 4 |
p. 307 |
“Roof” instead of ‘ceiling’ via a sound sequence: “room” → “roof” |
F ’71 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 429 p. 430 |
Error: “roof” instead of ‘ceiling’: common occurrence: recipients focus on it and…correct it. A piece of research: see whether if someone produces a possible error, others later exhibit that they picked up on it, though they didn’t say anything at the time. |
Lecture 12 |
p. 491 |
Language errors via over-application of a rule |
Etiquette |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 7, 8 |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 04.a |
p. 284 |
|
Lecture 04.b |
p. 297 |
||
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 61 p. 64 |
|
W ’70 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 208 |
|
S ’71 |
April 30 |
p. 380 |
|
May 21 |
p. 408 |
Evidence |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 24-25 |
|
Lecture 13 |
p. 105 |
‘Ex-Relationals’ |
|||
Volume II |
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 462-463 |
Exemplary Occurrences |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 11 |
pp. 196-198 |
Explanations [See Accountables, Accounts, Explanations, etc.] |
“Face-to-face” Materials, Observations, etc. |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 7 |
p. 50-51 |
Cigarette-lighting |
Lecture 12 |
p. 102-103 |
Physical co-presence → conversation |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 6 |
p. 673 |
“Eye-monitoring” for speaker-selection in multi-party conversation |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 65-66 |
Physical movements prior to being introduced; timing of eye contact |
W ’69 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 91 |
Looking at speaker → noticing something about them |
Lecture 8
|
pp. 130-131 |
Re-arranging physical proximities |
|
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 167 |
Noticing changes in someone’s appearance, possessions, etc. (Private calendar) |
Lecture 4 |
p. 193 |
Approaching parties withholding greetings until such a point as they can achieve a greeting-only interaction. |
|
S ’71 |
May 10 |
pp. 394-395 |
Male doctor lighting female patient’s cigarette |
S ’72 |
Lecture 6
|
pp. 571-572 |
Other occurrences during a ‘laughing together’: change color, move, look at each other |
Fitted Talk |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 4 |
Greetings |
S ’66 |
Lecture 6 |
p. 313 |
Reciprocal Identification |
W ’67 |
March 9 |
p. 537 |
2nd stories |
F ’67 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 675-676 |
‘Language shifts’ in ‘utterance pairs’ |
Lecture 9 |
p. 693 |
||
S ’68 |
April 17 |
pp. 753-754 |
Characterizations fitted to topic (e.g. “The woman who lives there now”; e.g. “across the street” from Bullock’s) |
April 24 |
pp. 769-771 |
2nd stories |
|
May 8 |
pp. 781-782 |
2nd stories |
|
May 29 |
pp. 795-797 |
2nd stories |
|
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 112-113 |
Fitted actions show understanding |
Foreshortened / Expanded Sequences |
|||
Volume II |
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 159-169 |
Re. greeting sequences |
Formulation |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 68-69 |
|
Lecture 10 |
p. 78 |
||
Lecture 12 |
p. 103 |
A 1st conversation as a version of an Nth |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 6 |
p. 166 |
‘We’ formulated in terms of ‘you’ |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 180-181 |
||
Lecture 10 |
pp. 190-191 |
||
Lecture 14 |
pp. 205-207 |
Alternative categorical formulations |
|
W ’67 |
February 16 |
pp. 515-522 |
Vis-à-vis ‘settings’ |
March 9 |
pp. 543-544 |
No room in the world for ‘just formulating’ |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 637-638 |
No free room for ‘just formulating’ |
S ’68 |
April 17 |
pp. 753-754 |
Characterizations of persons, places and ‘age class’ by reference to the topic at hand. |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 46 |
Not a sharp enough characterization, i.e. that “X is a formulation” |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 73-74 |
‘Orientational’ formulations |
|
W ’69 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 126-129 pp. 133-135 |
‘Identification reformulations’ ‘Abstract’ vs. ‘concrete’ formulations |
W ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 181 |
‘Gifts’ vs. ‘giftware’ (“formulation of who it is that’s talking to whom”) |
S ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 218-219 |
Banal characterizations: “It was nothing much”, “It was outta sight.” |
W ’71 |
February 19 |
pp. 293-294 |
“Parties” (vs. e.g. groups) & “giftware” as terminology by personnel, not clientele |
March 4 |
p. 309 |
Alternative formulations for expressing positions on reported event (“had to go” vs. “said he had to go”) |
|
S ’71 |
April 23 |
pp. 367-369 |
Re. “Talk, you mean get drunk…” ‘Correct partial formulations’ |
April 26 |
p. 371 p. 374 |
‘Going out with parents’ as “an altogether drastic reformulation” of ‘going out and getting drunk’ “…what some evenings are to be charaterizably devoted to, without regard to how anyone happens to spend an evening.” |
|
May 21 |
pp. 404-405 |
“Mr. Jones came into the room” vs. “he came into the room” |
|
May 24 |
p. 413 |
Post-“What?” reformulation |
|
F ’71 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 463-464 |
|
Lecture 15 |
pp. 507-509 |
Freedom of Occurrence |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 6 |
“I can’t hear you”, “Huh?” as ‘occasionally usable’ device: can come at any place |
F ’65 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 183 |
Freedom of positioning of clauses |
Lecture 14 |
p. 209 p. 213 |
Freedom of positioning of clauses In choosing 1st person in ‘a series’ of introductions |
|
Volume II |
|||
W ’71 |
February 19 |
p. 293 |
Freedom of occurrence of expletives |
F ’71 |
Lecture 11 |
p. 485 |
Freedom of occurrence of jokes |
S ’72 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 534-535 |
‘Freedom of occurrence rule’ for adjacency pair first pair parts. |
Games / Play |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 7 |
(Imagine)…skipping a move |
Lecture 6 |
p. 48 |
As “model conflict situations” |
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 87 |
Poker as a model for currencies |
|
Lecture 13 |
pp. 106-107 |
‘Families’ of games |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 160-161 |
Insult games, playing the dozens |
Lecture 10 |
p. 191 |
Hotrodders vs. cops |
|
Lecture 11 |
pp. 193-194 |
Schelling’s war games |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 307 |
Re. “I’m a military man” |
Lecture 12 |
pp. 360-362 |
Hotrodders vs. cops (countable successes) |
|
Lecture 13 |
pp. 363-369 |
Button-Button |
|
Lecture 31 |
pp. 473-478 |
Legal and illegal actions in games |
|
Appendix A |
pp. 489-506 |
‘On some formal properties of children’s games’ |
|
W ’67 |
March 2 |
pp. 531-532 |
Playing at challenges |
F ’67 |
Lecture 7 |
p. 679 |
Playing the dozens |
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 105-107, 110 p. 112 |
Re. “Turn on the microphone” Imitative play |
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 161 |
“Voice recognition games” |
S ’72 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 550 |
Voice “recognition games” |
Getting Something Done Without ‘Doing’ It |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 4 p. 7 |
Getting someone to give their name (providing a slot vs. asking) |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 15-16 |
Avoid giving helps (treat trouble as a joke) |
|
Lecture 10 |
p. 76 |
Providing that help will be offered without being asked for. (request for information) [cf. “Can you fix this needle?”] |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 04.b |
p. 293 |
Mistreating someone (not doing to them what is being done in a round) |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 324-325 |
Hotrodders vs. cops |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 73-74 |
Inviting-rejecting a new entrant via formulation of topic |
W ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 176-178 pp. 180-181 |
Conveying information without requiring a response (“Did you have the day off?” and “Mac…put [your newspaper] up on your porch” Conveying a scurrilous suggestion without being nasty |
S ’70 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 272-274 |
“Boy there goes a great gal”: dealing with awhile-ago events; utterances “specifically intended to be connected in some way, while also having their connectedness not directly available in other ways.” |
Lecture 8 |
p. 284 |
Where saying that an unfair question is unfair, is problematic, you can, by your answer, make it, show it to be, an unfair question. (Hippie producer) |
|
S ’71 |
May 24 |
p. 413 |
“What?” can get a clarification without disclosing lack of understanding (can pass as the ‘preferred’ hearing problem) |
F ’71 |
Lecture 6 |
p. 455 |
Indicating that something was “normal” without saying it. |
Glancing |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 7 |
p. 50 |
|
Lecture 11 |
pp. 81-94 |
Gossip |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’67 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 639-640 |
|
Lecture 10 |
pp. 703-704 |
||
S ’68 |
May 8 |
p. 776 |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 52-53 |
“People in that neighborhood are rude” |
W ’69 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 132 |
Greetings |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 4 |
|
Lecture 12 |
pp. 96-99 p. 97 |
No rule of exclusion |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 04.a |
pp. 284-285 |
Greeting substituted vis-à-vis ‘no-naming’ |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 308-309 |
‘Ahistorically relevant’ / no rule of exclusion |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 551-554 p. 554 |
‘Ahistorically relevant’ / no rule of exclusion ‘Greeting substitute’ |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 35-36 |
No exclusion rule / “remain relevant” |
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 158-169 |
‘Greeting sequence’ (foreshortened / expanded) |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 188-199 |
||
S ’72 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 544 |
Answerer’s “Hello” as a “greeting” by a possible ‘Called’ to a possible to-called-caller. |
Groups |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 145-146 |
|
Lecture 6 |
p. 168 |
||
Lecture 7 |
pp. 169-170 |
||
Lecture 8 |
pp. 175-176 |
||
F ’67 |
Lecture 10 |
p. 703 |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 65 |
Introducing someone to ‘members of a group’ |
Lecture 6
|
p. 78 pp. 78-79 |
‘Member’ talk: asserting a variable without stating its value (e.g. “It’s a 1950.” (cf. pp. 235-236, “Competence” via “usualness measures” vs. “more precise characterizations”) Simmel’s notion of ‘completeness’ |
|
W ’69 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 121-123 |
Membership in “kids’ groups” (hotrodders, hippies) |
Happenstance vs. Systematic |
|||
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 3-4 |
Re. 2nd stories’ similarity to 1sts |
Lecture 2 |
p. 25 |
Re. one is incidental, 2 maybe not |
|
W ’71 |
February 19 |
p. 292 |
Re. poetics phenomena |
March 4 |
p. 305 |
Re. 2nd stories similarity to 1sts |
|
S ’71 |
April 30 |
p. 377 |
Re. SPC caller not giving her name |
May 21 |
p. 404 |
Re. a replicated triplet: the 1st at start of call, the 2nd at close. |
Hell as a Mnemonic Technique |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’68 |
April 17 |
pp. 759-760 |
|
Volume II |
|||
S ’71 |
May 17 |
p. 399 |
‘Helping’ |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’68 |
April 29 |
pp. 788-789 |
Hinting |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 1 (R) |
pp. 249-250 |
“I was a hair stylist…” |
Lecture 08 |
pp. 329-332 |
||
S ’67 |
Lecture 12 |
pp. 578-581 |
“I was a hair stylist” etc. etc. |
Lecture 14 |
p. 595 |
A seeable ‘hint; a ‘hint’ in a double sense (“What’s new, gentlemen?”) |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 431-435 |
Allusive talk: sex |
Identification (See also ‘Categories & Classes’) |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 5-6 |
Stranger-stranger (vs. helper-helper) |
S ’66 |
Lecture 04.a |
pp. 289-290 |
‘Sufficiency of an identification’ problem (“Who’s Jim Reed?” / “Who’s Fido?”) |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 306-307 |
‘Proffering an identification’ |
|
Lecture 6 |
p. 313 pp. 317-319 |
‘Proffering an identification’ ‘Cover identifications’ |
|
Lecture 7 |
pp. 326-327 |
‘Unique solution’: via category bound to activity (cf. pp. 714-715) (“That’s Una’s mother”) Nth person solution: relational pairs. |
|
Lecture 08 |
pp. 328-332 |
Orientation: ‘phony’ / hinting |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 362 |
Via. Making a complaint |
|
Lecture 18 |
p. 399 |
‘Uniquely-selected’ identification category via category bound to activity. |
|
Lecture 21 |
pp. 417-420 |
‘Intentional misidentification’ |
|
W ’67 |
March 2 |
p. 532 |
“Possible identifications invocable in a sense” |
March 9 |
p. 544 |
‘Intentional misidentification’ (broken down into intentional mis-address; ‘intentional mis-reference’.) |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 12 |
pp. 580-581 |
‘Hints at identification’ |
Lecture 13
|
pp. 588-589 |
Selecting identifications via category bound activities |
|
Lecture 14
|
pp. 592-594 |
‘Cover’ identifications |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 676-680 |
‘Intentional mis-address’ |
Lecture 011 |
pp. 712-713 |
Focusing on relevance of an identification category |
|
S ’68 |
April 17 |
p. 753 |
Identifications as ‘’topic carriers’: people & places |
May 8 |
pp. 774-775 |
Request and rejection via identification ‘stewardess’ |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 6 |
p. 73 |
Vis-à-vis orientation formulations |
W ’69 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 126-129 |
‘Identification reformulation’ |
W ’70 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 201-202 |
Identity transformation |
W ’71 |
March 11 |
pp. 327-330 |
Identity transformation |
May 21 |
pp. 404-405 |
“Mr. Jones” vs. “he” (Virginia Woolf) |
|
F ’71 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 444-452 |
Selecting identifications; preference for Type I over Type II identification |
Lecture 8 |
p. 466 |
Identity transformation |
|
Lecture 10 |
p. 482 |
Re. “a series of identities”, where ‘mother-daughter’ is “crucial”. |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 490 |
Re. “a series of identities”, where ‘mother-daughter’ is “crucial”. |
|
S’72 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 542-553 |
‘Caller-Answerer-Called’ etc. |
Ideology |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 70 |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 14 |
p. 214 |
|
Volume II |
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 187 |
‘The ideological foundations of perception’ |
Idioms, Tokens, etc. |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 8-10 |
“May I help you” |
F ’65 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 156 |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 212, 220 |
“I don’t lose any sleep over it” |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 12 |
pp. 358-359 |
“That’s the problem with society.” |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 58 |
“Through circumstances” “beyond their control” |
S ’71 |
May 17 |
pp. 396-397 |
Various spatialized idioms in SPC:NYE call |
F ’71 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 421 |
“They need…something to look up to.” |
Images |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 4 p. 7 |
“Multi-dimensional jigsaw puzzle” Game |
Lecture 13 |
p. 111 |
Human language, as “alike to that of other animals”: narrative commentary |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 159 |
“The warehouse” |
S ’66 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 237-238 |
“The little tiny things that God might have forgotten” |
Lecture 11 |
pp. 348-349 |
“Interruption and completion” involves passing a sentence like a relay race |
|
Lecture 21 |
p. 425 |
“Warehouse” |
|
Lecture 33 |
p. 484 |
“A machine with a couple of holes in front…spew[ing] out garbage” from the back. |
|
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 138 |
“In the worn out stone that was a statue; you can see a smoothed down face…” |
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 169 |
“Over aim is to…transform, in almost a literal, physical sense, one view of what happen[s]…to interactions being spewed out by machinery, the machinery being what we’re trying to find” |
Lecture 3 |
p. 240 |
Machinery produces orderly events, but most orderly events are byproducts. |
|
S ’70 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 282 |
“You could always figure if [a question] is one you mind it’s in other people’s minds. It’s like a fly that’s moving around the room and now it’s ion your shoulder; it just happened to settle there…” |
W ’71 |
March 4 |
p. 313 |
Delicate perspectives, “like a seven-layer cake or a flickering candle, get passed on for generations as a reasonable characterization of the world, without getting smashed…” |
F ’71 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 428 |
Duck / rabbit illusions vis-à-vis proverbial expressions’ empirical / proverbial sense vis-à-vis unnoticed puns. |
Lecture 8 |
p. 469 |
“It’s as thought the stories in people’s heads are more or less constantly alert for the occasions for which they are distinctly apt.” |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 493 |
Dirty jokes “can move within the groups…informing its members of [matters] of distinct interest to just those groups, [as], e.g., private newsletters for small or large stockholders can do that kind of a job, or a motorcycle magazine can do that for motorcyclists.” |
|
S ’72 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 548-549 p. 553 |
Re. introduction of the telephone. “Now what happens is, like any other natural object, a culture secretes itself onto it in its well-shaped ways…This technical apparatus is, then, being made at home with the rest of our world.” Answerer’s ‘Hello’, “if it exhibits an emotion; exhibits an emotion to the world… So that it’s a flair sent up as compared to an aimed emotion.” |
Imitation |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 70-71 |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 31 |
p. 477 |
|
Lecture 32 |
pp. 479-482 |
||
Appendix A |
pp. 503-504 |
||
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 111-112 |
Importance, Interestingness, etc. |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 7 |
Rules of etiquette |
Lecture 4 |
p. 28 |
Kings, votes, revolution |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 04.b |
p. 293 |
Scandal, discourse of kings, etc. vs. greetings |
Lecture 6 |
p. 312 |
‘Deviance’, ‘failure of integration’, ‘role strains’ |
|
Lecture 23 |
pp. 428-430 |
Where is language deep and interesting, “What not to study” |
|
Lecture 30 |
pp. 470-471 pp. 471-472 |
Why do scientists do science? ‘Attitudes toward authority’ (Student Q) |
|
Lecture 33 |
p. 483ff |
Finding “good” problems: crime rate, bank loan rate, political order, etc. |
|
W ’67 |
February 16 |
p. 522 |
Jet set, Pentagon Chart Room…4 kids on a Saturday morning…All settings are equivalent. |
March 9 |
pp. 541-542 |
‘One’s life’ / ‘a promise’ vs. ‘embarassability’/‘respect for topical organization’ |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 563 |
“Everyone has to lie” sounds interesting, “everyone is in a position to have to choose whether to lie or not” sounds uninteresting |
F ’67 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 664 |
“The things in the world that are going to count theoretically…will not necessarily come with labels on them: ‘Look at me, I’m really important.’” |
Lecture 14 |
p. 741 p. 743 |
“Why do people stop at stoplights?” & “Why don’t people rape their neighbors?” vs. “You are only at the point where sociology is interesting when you can see that ‘Tuesday’ is the right sort of answer…and ‘November eleventh’…would get you committed.” “The sheer appropriateness of such an answer as ‘Tuesday’ beguiles you into figuring that there could be nothing interesting present in its being done.” |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 57 |
Interestingness of collaborative utterances |
Lecture 5 |
p. 62 |
‘Nothing much’ phenomenon can be an object for regulation (upper-class English return whatever greeting they’re offered.) |
|
W ’69 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 133-134 |
“Large scale interest” of possible preferences for concrete (vs. abstract) conversation |
W ’70 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 204 |
“Really interesting mechanisms going on” in “these incredibility nothing-happening happenings of conversation” |
S ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 215 |
“What sort of large-scale interest does what people make stories of or what they don’t make stories of, have?” |
Lecture 2 |
p. 226 |
Story preface as ‘interest arouses’ |
|
Lecture 4 |
pp. 247-248 |
Locating the importance of a story’s events |
|
W ’71 |
March 11 |
p. 325 |
Treating the ‘poetics’ phenomenon as ‘really outrageously’ important |
F ’71 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 420 |
Interesting aspects of the world, that are as yet unknown, are accessible to observation. |
Lecture 8 |
p. 467 |
“A question that can be either banal or deep…is this: Why do people transmit information to others?” |
|
S ’72 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 542 |
“There is a rule for telephone-call beginning which may sound awfully trivial but has turned out to have varieties of interesting theoretical implications. |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 554-555 |
“A lot of this [consideration of ‘next position’] will sound awfully banal but it’s far from that…it’s not, after all, something anyone could have said; it’s not that it’s nothing; it’s not that it has no consequences.” |
|
Lecture 5 |
pp. 562-563 |
Re. talk about disasters goes at the beginning of conversations. Specification: if introduceable as a “How are you” – type questions. “And that little specification may turn out to wag the disasters.” |
Incongruity |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 11 |
pp. 89-90, 92 |
Indexical Expressions |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 32 |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 148-149 |
‘We’ |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 150-154 |
||
Lecture 6 |
pp. 164-168 |
“You”… ‘We’ |
|
Lecture 7 |
p. 169 |
‘We’ |
|
Lecture 9 |
p. 182 |
||
Lecture 14 |
p. 206 |
‘You’ |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 333-336 |
‘We’ |
Lecture 10 |
pp. 342-343 |
Pro-verbs |
|
Lecture 11 |
pp. 348-353 |
‘You’…‘We’…‘it’ |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 374-375 |
Re. Tying Rules |
|
Lecture 15 |
pp. 376-378 p. 377 |
Re. Tying Rules Pro-verbs |
|
Lecture 16 |
pp. 382-383, 387-388 |
Possessive pronouns |
|
Lecture 29 |
pp. 461-463 |
Place references (‘here’, etc.) |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 11 |
pp. 568-575 |
‘We’ |
Lecture 16 |
pp. 605-606 |
Possessive pronoun ‘my’ |
|
Lecture 17 |
pp. 613-615 |
Pro-verbs |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 7 |
p. 675 |
Pronouns are not substitutes for nouns |
Lecture 011 |
pp. 711-715 |
Various pronouns (“tying phenomenon”) |
|
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Fragment |
pp. 153-154 |
Pronoun use in early logic |
W ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 182 |
“They” |
W ’71 |
February 19 |
p. 291 |
“WE went [to a bar] and…THEY had an after dinner drink.” |
S ’71 |
April 26 |
pp. 373-374 |
Ambiguity of ‘you’ as to singular / plural reference. |
May 10 |
p. 391 |
“We” & “They” are not only plural references, but also…‘organizational references’ |
|
May 21 |
pp. 404-405 |
“He” to identify someone entering, in Virginia Woolf novels |
|
F ’71 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 445-446 |
Pronoun & relational terms vis-à-vis recipient design |
Indirect Actions (See ‘Getting something done without doing it’) |
Volume I |
Volume II |
Inference |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 14 |
p. 115ff |
Definition: “Deal[ing] with, categoriz[ing] and mak[ing] statements about an event [one] has not seen.” |
Volume II |
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 6 |
p. 457 |
Re. “warding off inferences that could be made from [a] specific event [being described].” |
Informants |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 27 |
Information: Packaging and Transmission |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 13 |
pp. 110-111 |
|
Lecture 14 |
p. 116 |
||
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 69-71 |
In Introductions |
W ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 176-178 pp. 180-181 |
Conveying information without requiring a response to it (here, doing a favor without requiring “Thanks”; answering return of husband without making it a topic
Conveying information via ‘elaborating a recognition type description’ |
F ’71 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 466-469 |
Preserving and transmitting knowledge via stories |
Lecture 11 |
pp. 485-486 |
Dirty jokes: ‘pass with discretion’ |
|
Lecture 12 |
pp. 489-490 pp. 492-494 |
Sexual information packaged in dirty joke Dirty joke as “a private newsletter” |
Innocence (See Playing Dumb) |
Volume I |
Volume II |
Institutional Use of Ordinary Devices |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 8 |
“May I help you” |
Lecture 2 |
p. 22 |
Police use correction-invitation device |
|
Volume II |
|||
S ’71 |
April 30 |
p. 380 |
“May I help you” |
Insults |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 160-161 |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 11 |
p. 351 |
|
Lecture 21 |
p. 419 |
||
Lecture 26 |
p. 446 |
||
F ’67 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 678-679 |
Closing off an insult sequence |
Volume II |
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 276-280 |
|
S ’71 |
April 26 |
pp. 374-375 |
You can be offended at how someone figures you spend Saturday night even if you spend it in an even feebler way than what they’d proposed. (talk vs. get drunk vs. going to dinner with parents). |
Interruption (See ‘Turn-taking) |
Volume I |
Volume II |
Intimacy |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 14 pp. 17-19 |
‘A special class of others’ Invitations no longer relevant |
Lecture 10 |
pp. 73-74 |
Accounts of how one came to the call; such accounts not used between intimates |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 560-562 |
Restrictions on exchange of ‘personal state’ information |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 50 |
‘Interruption’ as index of intimacy |
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 162 |
Re. call scheduling |
Lecture 4 |
p. 196 |
Between strangers doing tragedy / disaster |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 204 |
Re. telephone call openings |
|
S ’70 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 248 |
“Even…what you thought was a friend” will require an “anybody” storytelling |
W ’71 |
March 4 |
p. 311 |
‘Intimate complaining’ |
S ’71 |
May 21 |
pp. 405-406 |
Caller to SPC apologizing to the professional for the “bother” “is focusing on that we might be intimates but we’re not.” |
F ’71 |
Lecture 15 |
p. 511 |
Intimate problem (family) talked about conventionally, unintimately. |
S ’72 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 544 |
“Intimate conversation of sorts” between caller and phone-answerer-not-called |
Intonation |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 14 |
p. 373 |
Question-intonation? |
W ’67 |
March 9 |
p. 543 |
Systematically characterizable activities “I did”, “I did” |
F ’67 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 651 |
“Intonation contour” |
Lecture 13 |
pp. 735-736 |
Accent (emphasis); e.g. “I did”, “I did” “No let’s take my car” |
|
S ’68 |
May 29 |
p. 788 |
“I thought X” vs. “I thought X” |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 22 |
“I did”, “I did” |
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 160, 162 |
“Yeah” vs. “Yeah?” by called, to “[Name]?” |
Lecture 5 |
p. 203 |
“Yeah” vs. “Yeah?” by called, to “[Name]?” |
|
S ’72 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 526 |
Question intonation in the course of an utterance; coparticipant does “Uh huh” |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 558-559 |
Intonation can position an utterance (e.g. doubting “We:::ll,” e.g. contrast stressing.) |
Introductions |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 148 |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 04.a |
pp. 281-291 |
|
Lecture 04.b |
pp. 292-299 |
||
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 69-71 |
Invitations |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 18-19 |
|
Lecture 10 |
p. 73 |
||
F ’65 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 175-176 |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 301-305 |
|
Lecture 6 |
pp. 312-313 |
||
Lecture 29 |
pp. 465-466 |
||
S ’68 |
May 29 |
pp. 791-793 |
Interactionally generated invitations |
Volume II |
|||
W’ 70 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 210-211 |
Interactionally generated invitations |
S ’71 |
April 23 |
pp. 367-368 |
‘First-preference invitations’ |
S ’72 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 528-529 |
Invitations as ‘first pair parts’ |
Lecture 6 |
p. 571 p. 575 |
Re. “whether laugh offerings are accepted or not.” Interactionally generated invitations |
Job’s Problem |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 20 |
|
Lecture 14 |
p. 118 |
||
S ’66 |
Lecture 20 |
p. 412 |
Jokes, Joke-Serious Alternation, etc. |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 12-16 p. 16 |
Treating announcement of suicidalness as a joke Joking response to “How are you?” |
Lecture 12 |
pp. 99-100 |
Sequential character of |
|
|
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 04.b |
pp. 294-295 |
Jokes come in rounds |
Lecture 27 |
p. 451 |
Obscene jokes / “a chaining of humor” |
|
|
|||
F ’67 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 682-684 |
Announcing that a joke is forthcoming |
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 131 |
Laughing at someone’s joke to show sexual interest in taller. |
|
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 205-206 |
Joking relationships |
|
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 470-477 |
The dirty joke as a technical object |
Lecture 10 |
pp. 478-482 |
The dirty joke as a technical object |
|
Lecture 11
|
pp. 483-488 |
The dirty joke as a technical object |
|
Lecture 12
|
pp. 489-494 |
The dirty joke as a technical object |
Knowledge |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 8 |
“I don’t know” |
Lecture 3 |
p. 23 |
Common knowledge |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 33 |
“I don’t know” |
|
Lecture 6 |
pp. 40-42, 46 |
||
Lecture 9 |
p. 69 |
||
Lecture 10 |
p. 83 |
‘Feigning ignorance’ |
|
Lecture 13 |
pp. 109-110 |
||
F ’65 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 180 |
‘Protected against induction’ |
Lecture 11 |
pp. 196, 198 |
‘Protected against induction’ |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 202 |
Lay vs. scientific |
|
Lecture 14 |
p. 222 |
Greek “know thyself” as not private |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 336-340 |
‘Protected against induction’ |
Lecture 14 |
p. 374 |
“I don’t know” |
|
Lecture 17 |
pp. 394-395 |
‘Protected against induction’ (scientists’ sins as otherwise reasonable procedures.) |
|
Lecture 19 |
p. 406 |
How do you know your own appearance? |
|
Lecture 30 |
pp. 470-471 |
Why do scientists do science? |
|
Volume II |
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 244-245 |
“Stock of knowledge” vs. “stock of experiences” |
F ’71 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 466-469 |
Preserving & transmitting knowledge via stories (vis-à-vis one’s own experiences) |
Laughing |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’67 |
Lecture 14 |
pp. 745-746 |
“Laughing is one prototypical thing that people can be doing together…” |
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 131 |
Laughing at someone’s joke to indicate sexual interest |
W ’70 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 207 |
Getting someone ‘laughing along’ before doing touchy business (‘Power tools’) |
S ’70 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 274-276 p. 280 |
Pre-positioned “heh”s in a story |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 284-288 |
“heh Wh(hh)en I grow up! heh heh …” Heckling (stories) |
|
S ’71 |
May 10 |
p. 395 |
((Laughs)) “You’re making me laugh. I must be feeling better” |
F ’71 |
Lecture 11 |
p. 486 |
“Laughing in an appropriately timed way sufficiently indicates an appreciation of [a] joke.” |
S ’72 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 570-572 |
Laughing together |
Lay-Professional / Lay-Scientific, etc. |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 10 |
Diagnostic term “regressed” / lay affiliate |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 38-39 |
Re. non-conformance to a theory |
|
Lecture 6 |
p. 42 |
Lay theories of social actions |
|
Lecture 8 |
p. 59 |
‘Normal’ as “a medical device” |
|
Lecture 9 |
p. 69 |
[Lay] theory of how men select a mate |
|
Lecture 14 |
p. 115 p. 121 |
Lay notion: simple actions, simple explanations, vs. scientists trying to describe 6-year-olds’ talk, activities of molecules. Something has occurred, our job is to explain it; vs. (for possible lies, etc.) that possible-fact which has an explanation is the one that occurred. |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 146 |
Literary aid lay use of collaborative utterances |
Lecture 12 |
pp. 200-203 |
Lay use of psychiatric terms |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 304 |
First Five Minutes as “lay”-analytic |
S ’67 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 558 |
Lay psychology |
S ’68 |
April 17 |
p. 752 |
Lay approach to ‘topic’ |
Volume II |
|||
F ‘68 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 4 p. 8 p. 15 |
“Lay observation” Second stories are similar to the prior story “Anybody could say that” Lay formulations of time and place |
Lecture 2 |
p. 18 |
Re: Stories. Technical features of lay characterization |
|
S ‘70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 217 |
Laymen would not venture an opinion of chemistry or physics, but feel free to make psychological remarks (Freud) |
Lecture 6 |
p. 267 |
“as a lay matter”, noticing defensiveness. |
|
Lecture 7 |
p. 269-71 |
“What’s going on in a lay sense” |
|
S ‘71 |
April 9 |
p. 348 |
“Technical competition in conversation” |
Lies, Lying |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 10 |
p. 79 |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 114-115 |
||
F ’65 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 185 |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 255 |
|
Lecture 13 |
pp. 365-366 |
||
Lecture 20 |
pp. 411, 414 |
||
Lecture 26
|
pp. 448-449 |
||
S ’67 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 556 |
|
Lecture 9 |
pp. 563-565 |
||
S ’68 |
April 24 |
p. 772 |
|
Volume II |
|||
S ’71 |
April 30 |
p. 380 |
Suicidal callers to SPC will not give their names, but also will not lie. |
May 10 |
p. 394 |
Mr. Smith’s bluff called by suicidal caller. |
Linguistics |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 26ff
|
|
Lecture 12 |
pp. 95, 99 |
|
|
Lecture 13 |
pp. 107-108, 112 |
|
|
|
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 23 |
pp. 428-430 |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 5 |
|
Lists, Listing |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 148-149 |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 159 |
||
Lecture 7 |
p. 169 |
||
S ’66 |
Lecture 27 |
p. 450-451 |
Relevance of a list to the hearing of an nth |
F ’67 |
Lecture 12 |
pp. 726-727 |
Orientation to a list generates a puzzle |
S ’68 |
May 22 |
pp. 784-785 |
Speaking a list |
May 29 |
p. 800 |
Partitioned items on a list |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 13 |
X, Y “also” |
Lecture 5 |
p. 65 |
“And” placed before last item on a list |
|
W ’71 |
March 4 |
p. 316 |
“I don’t want an X, I want a Y or a Z.” |
S ’71 |
May 21 |
p. 404 |
A replicated triplet |
F ’71 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 475 |
At least and no more than 3 events needed to exhibit the last of them as peculiar |
Lecture 14 |
pp. 500-502 |
“I don’t want an X, I want a Y or a Z.” |
|
S ’72 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 562 p. 565 |
Lists as “ideal natural objects for getting at some sorts of commonness of meanings.” (re. “How did you survive the quake? And the fires and the floods and everything.”) Re. a question that contains a list can propose topicality vs. just Q-A. |
Literature |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 13 |
p. 112 |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 145, 146, 147 |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 242 |
“Computers could build novels” |
Lecture 7
|
p. 322 |
Masters, Freud, Donald Duck. |
|
S ’68 |
May 29 |
p. 791 |
|
Volume II |
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 216, 217 |
|
Lecture 3 |
pp. 238-239 |
What Gogol did to Western literature (de-economization) |
|
S ’71 |
May 21 |
pp. 404-405 |
What Virginia Woolf did to the novel (de-identification) |
Localizing the World |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 36-38 |
Private calendars |
S ’66 |
Lecture 28 |
p. 458-460 |
Local environment adequate for making comparisons & attributing causes |
S ’68 |
April 24 |
p. 770 |
Storyteller making an event (auto wreck) into something in her life. |
May 8 |
p. 780 |
“We’ll have an unusual experience to talk about” (1st day in concentration camp) |
|
May 29 |
p. 791 p. 794-795, 798 |
(“Privatizing the world”) (“Adequacy of very local environments”) |
|
April 17 |
pp. 753-754 |
“Two police cars across the street” |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 13-15 |
“Goshawful wreck” happened “on the way home” |
Lecture 2 |
p. 28 |
Making an event (auto wreck) into one’s own experience |
|
W ’69 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 92-93 |
‘Local resources’ for making conversation |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 134-135 |
“The collection of co-participants defines the population of ‘to whom [an utterance’s action] would be done.” |
|
S ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 215-221 |
Doing ‘being ordinary’ |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 243-248 |
Entitlement to experiences |
|
Lecture 5 |
pp. 258-259 |
Story experience in terms of your place in them |
|
W ’71 |
February 19 |
pp. 296-298 |
Adequacy of local collection of people as compliment / complaint recipients about whatsoever. |
F ’71 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 467-468 |
“People…are built to be the custodians of just about only their own experiences.” |
Lecture 11 |
pp. 483-484 |
NB data re. RFK assassination: “It would have ruined your whole trip” and kid’s home run “the only good thing that happened to me this week.” (i.e. “it is her life that the assassination has happened to.”) |
|
S ’72 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 548-549 p. 551 |
Re. making the telephone “at home with the rest of our world.” “Each new object becomes the occasion for seeing again what we can see anywhere; seeing people’s nastinesses or goodnesses and all the rest…” Re. telephone as source of family conflict |
Lecture 5 |
p. 563 pp. 568-569 |
Re. ‘disasters’ as a conversational topic: People talk “overwhelmingly…about things insofar as thing happened to them. Talking about whatever, it comes home to ‘us’.” Making events into talk by turning them into ‘something for us’. |
Logic |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 37 |
Counterfactual conditionals |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 66-67 |
Suicide possibility as “logical” |
|
Lecture 13 |
p. 112 |
And ‘potential descriptions’ |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 6 |
p. 164 |
Transiency of references: indexicals |
S ’66 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 333 |
And ‘indexical expressions’ |
W ’67 |
February 16 |
pp. 517-518 |
And ‘indicator terms’ |
S ’67 |
Lecture 11 |
p. 568 |
Vis-à-vis ‘contradictory statements’ |
F ’67 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 687 |
Re. paraphrased quotations of Q-A. |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 696-697 |
Re. the Stoic paradoxes |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 741-742 |
Re. “Tuesday” vs. “November 11, 1967.” |
Macro / Micro (See also ‘Importance, Interestingness, etc.’) |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 3 |
Agency’s concern / addressed question |
Lecture 2 |
p. 17 p. 19 |
‘Structure of society’ / focusing kinds of troubles “Comparative dynamics of the persons” / “Just examining an interchange” |
|
Lecture 3 |
pp. 22-23 p. 24 |
“A larger sense” (murder interrogation) / other matters Apparent complexity / possible simplicity |
|
Lecture 4 |
pp. 27-28 |
Durkheim: macro |
|
Lecture 8 |
p. 65 |
[Sacks addresses the terms ‘macro’ / ‘micro’] |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 7 |
p. 170 |
Institutional vs. Conversational decision |
Lecture 8 |
p. 178 |
Sacred vs. mundane |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 237-238 |
“Fine power of a culture” |
Lecture 1 (R) |
pp. 245-245 |
“Fine power of a culture” |
|
Lecture 33 |
pp. 483-486 |
Overview |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 14 |
p. 742 |
Relevancies of a detail (“Tuesday” vs. “November 11, 1967”) are so powerful and so extensive…“Not just stopping at the red light, but preceding properly with each detail.” |
S ’68 |
May 8 |
p. 783 |
2nd stories can be “gigantically consequential” |
May 29 |
p. 795 |
“Your friends are just as good to thank as the gods.” |
Meaning |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 13 |
p. 112 |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 12 |
pp. 200-201 |
Measurement |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 - S ’65 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 57-62 |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 24 |
pp. 435-440 |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 14 |
pp. 742-743 |
Approximate vs. precise numbers |
Volume II |
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 183 |
Approximate vs. precise time reference |
S ’70 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 235-236 |
“Usualness” measures vs. “more precise” characterizations [cf. F ’68 Lecture 6 p. 78, ‘member talk’: “asserting a variable without stating its value”] Here, to do with “competence”. |
Membership Categorization Device: (See ‘Categories & Classes’) |
Volume I |
Volume II |
Memory |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’68 |
April 17 |
pp. 759-760 p. 761 |
Place and memory Memory touched off by a word |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 4-8 |
Memory search for 2nd story |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 21-24 pp. 27-28 |
“I remember” proposes ‘topical coherence’ Remembering “in conversation terms”, amnesia |
|
S ’70 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 257-260 |
Memory & 2nd story; storing information, etc. |
W ’71 |
February 19 |
p. 299 |
Memory can be trusted to store & release items into conversation |
S ’71 |
April 12 |
p. 356 |
“Relative amnesia for specifically last topic” |
May 17 |
p. 399 pp. 400-401 |
Spatializing techniques and memory Memory & mind control |
|
F ’71 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 462-463 |
Memory for ‘famous ex-relationals’ [‘X-relationals’ ed.] |
Lecture 16 |
pp. 512-514 |
Remembering a dream vs. remembering a real event |
Minority Groups |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 42-48 |
|
Lecture 9 |
pp. 68, 70 |
||
Lecture 10 |
p. 74 |
||
Lecture 11 |
p. 82 |
||
F ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 160 |
|
Lecture 7 |
pp. 171-174 |
||
Lecture 10 |
p. 191 |
||
S ’66 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 336-340 |
|
Lecture 18 |
pp. 396-403 |
||
Lecture 26 |
pp. 446-449 |
||
Lecture 28 |
pp. 458-459 |
||
Lecture 32 |
pp. 479-480 |
||
S ’67 |
Lecture 11 |
p. 577 |
|
Lecture 12 |
pp. 582 |
||
Lecture 13 |
p. 586 |
||
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 118-123 |
|
W ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 180-181 pp. 185-187 |
|
Lecture 4 |
pp. 198-199 |
||
S ’70 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 282-284 |
|
F ’71 |
Lecture 7 |
p. 459 (data) |
“Kirk Douglas is Jewish too” |
S ’72 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 538, 539 |
“Unique position”: the oldest in the class, by far; the only black, woman, cop in the class, etc. “How do you get along with…all those others who are younger, white, male, etc.” |
M.I.R. Device |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 40-48 |
|
Lecture 9 |
pp. 68-69 |
Moves (See also Turn-taking, Sequence) |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 6-7 |
Skipping a move |
Lecture 12 |
p. 96 |
Proper ‘move’: a greeting after a greeting |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 160 |
|
Lecture 9 |
p. 184 |
||
Lecture 10 |
p. 190 |
||
Volume II |
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 160 |
|
Lecture 9 |
pp. 21-24 pp. 27-28 |
||
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 159, 162 |
Greeting sequence by analogy to a chess game |
Names, No-Naming, etc. |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 04.a |
pp. 284-285 pp. 289-290 |
No-naming Names as insufficient identification |
Lecture 20 |
pp. 414-416 |
Names as “disguised descriptions” |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 12 |
p. 724 |
Names: predictably wrongly transcribed |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 61-62 pp. 63-64 |
No-naming Name-types in introductions |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 68-69 |
Name-types in introductions |
|
W ’69 |
Lecture 7 |
p. 115 pp. 117-118 |
Motor → engine → “thing” (no-naming) “Thunderbird” vs. “Ford” / a car named Voodoo vs. “I call my TV Charlie” |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 146-149 |
Place names |
|
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 160, 162 p. 161 |
Caller does “[Name]?” to get to 1st topic Name types in phone call openings |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 203, 204 |
Caller does “[Name]?” to get to 1st topic |
|
S ’71 |
April 30 |
pp. 377-380 |
SPC caller not giving her name |
News |
|||
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 11-16 |
“Goshawful accident” as candidate ‘local news’ |
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 172-173 |
What happened at Bullocks today as ‘potential news’ |
S ’70 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 230-231, 238 |
“Goshawful accident” as possible local news |
S ’72 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 563-564 |
Non-local character of events that are ‘news’. Turning a disaster into “something for us.” |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 572-574 |
Announcements of “big news” followed by “expression of surprise” by recipient. |
Normal, Standard, Routine, Normative, etc. |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 9-10
pp. 9-10
|
“May I help you” as ‘standardized utterance’
Routine treatment |
Lecture 2 |
p. 19
|
“Normal answer to [X] is [Y].” |
|
Lecture 3 |
p. 22
|
“Routinely used” device |
|
Lecture 8 |
pp. 58-59
p. 62
|
‘Normal’ as a ‘standardized category’
‘Normal events’ vs. ‘odd events’. |
|
Lecture 11 |
pp. 88-89
p. 93
|
‘Average’
“Routinely the case that persons know what others are thinking.” |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 96
p. 103
|
‘Greeting’ as ‘standardized’ ACU
‘Standardized’ objects to start conversation |
|
|
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 11 |
p. 195
|
“A routine argument: if you do X, Y is bound to happen” |
|
|||
S ‘66 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 310-311 |
‘normative pause’ |
|
|||
F ’67
|
Lecture 4 |
p. 649
|
‘The sentence as a normative production’ |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 699-700
|
Extraordinary statements (e.g. in GTS, Ken’s paradox) as otherwise routine (also, see Puns) |
|
Lecture 14 |
p. 740 |
Routine, mundane matters
|
|
|
|||
S ’68 |
April 24 |
p. 767
p. 771
p. 772
|
Story-characterizing adjectives as “normative phenomena between groups.”
“Normative status” of the search technique for a 2nd story.
Pacing rules as “powerful normative techniques”. |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 50
|
Turn-taking rules as ‘normative’ |
Lecture 6 |
p. 67
|
Introductions as ‘normative’ |
|
|
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 159
p. 159
p. 170
|
Greeting sequences “characteristically”, “generally” 6 or 7 utterances long.
“Utterly normative first move”: ‘Hello’ “essentially universal”
‘Reason-for-call’ status “…seems to have normative classificatory importance”
|
|
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 230
p. 235
|
“…it is utterly routine for stories, that tellers [recipient-design them]…”
‘Usualness or normalness measures” [of ‘member talk’: asserting a variable without stating its value (e.g. “It’s a 1950!”) Volume II p. 78 F ’68 Lecture 6.]
|
|
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 419
|
Re. limitations of proving hypothetical data as “typical” |
Lecture 6 |
p. 455
p. 457
|
As part of a story, teller indicates that the sex occurred at its “normal” temporal position for a date.
Commitment to the “normal preferences” |
|
Lecture 9 |
p. 476
|
Re. “the normal form for talk “across a series of reported utterances” |
Norms |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 10 |
p. 77
|
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 93
|
|
|
|
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 10 |
p. 190
|
|
Lecture 14 |
p. 206
|
|
|
|
|||
S ’66
|
Lecture 2 |
pp. 253-254
|
|
Lecture 2 (R) |
p. 260
|
|
|
Lecture 18 |
p. 399
|
|
|
|
|||
W ’67 |
March 9 |
p. 545 |
To ‘direct reference’ the “norm”?
|
|
|||
F ’67 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 634
|
|
Lecture 4 |
p. 649
|
The sentence as a ‘normative production’ |
|
Lecture 14 |
p. 740
|
A ‘normative feature’ of paraphrases |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 21
|
“There’s’ a major sort of norm against repeating the same thing to the same persons.”
|
Obligation (See ‘Rights and Obligations’) |
Volume I |
Volume II |
Observability / Observables, etc. (See also Recognizablity) |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 28, 31 |
|
Lecture 8 |
p. 58 |
‘Noticeable’ events |
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 88 pp. 90-91 |
Of such units as ‘a family’ |
|
Lecture 13 |
p. 107 |
‘Noticeable’ events |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 119-121 |
For Members, activities are observables |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 10 |
pp. 190, 191 |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 04.a |
p. 283 p. 287 |
An observably orderly sequence Observability of a ‘second’ activity |
Lecture 04.b |
p. 293-295 |
Observable absences |
|
Lecture 12 |
pp. 363-368 |
Observable thoughts (Button-Button) |
|
Appendix A |
p. 493 p. 506 |
Observable game events Observable errors |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 4 |
Observable-to-coparticipant similarity of 2nd story to 1st |
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 174 |
Relevance of a ‘crowd watching’ to observability of an event |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 185-187 |
“Differential organization of the sheer perceiving of an event” (Estelle & Bullocks) |
|
Lecture 4 |
pp. 194-195 |
Relevance of ‘crowd’ to observability of an event |
|
S ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 215-221 |
Achieving the observed ordinariness of events |
S ’72 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 539 |
That one is the oldest in the class “by far” is ‘glance-determinable’. |
Offers |
|||
Volume II |
|||
W ’71 |
March 11 |
pp. 318-331 |
A re-offer sequence (‘herring’) |
“On the Way Home” = ‘The Story is about to Come’ |
|||
Volume II |
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 232 |
Opinion |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 33 |
Overall Structural Organization |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’66 |
|
p. 309 |
Pacing |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’68
|
April 24 |
pp. 771-772
|
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 27-28
|
Remembering in “conversation time” |
|
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 166, 168
|
Solving a problem “within utterance time” |
|
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 257, 259
|
Remembering in “conversation time” |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 266-267
|
Producing a solution to a posed problem “on the spur of the moment” |
|
|
|||
S ’71 |
April 5 |
p. 341
pp. 346-347
|
Selecting words “within conversation time”
Finding ‘best possible attack’ in ‘no time’ |
May 21 |
pp. 410-411
|
“Uh huh” vis-à-vis no gap no overlap, within 1/10 second |
|
|
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 428-429
|
Producing an understanding of a story “right now”… “which happens to be 2 seconds in this case.” (GTS “roof” (ceiling) / “something to look up to”)
|
Pairing Off |
|||
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 127, 128, 129-133
|
No Nancy (NB data) & Deep South (ethnography) |
Pairs |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65
|
Lecture 1 |
p. 4
|
Greetings |
Lecture 2 |
p. 14
|
“How are you?” “Fine” |
|
Lecture 6 |
pp. 47-48
|
Two-set classes |
|
Lecture 12 |
pp. 96, 101
|
Greetings |
|
|
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 254
p. 256
|
Pairs of actions related by norms
Questions get answers |
Lecture 2(R) |
p. 260
p. 264
|
Pairs of action related by norms
Questions get answers |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 308-311
|
Greetings; Q-A |
|
Lecture 7 |
pp. 323-325
pp. 326-327
|
Paired utterances; ‘interruption’; ‘Long vs. Short Control’
Relational pair categories |
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 351
|
Insult-Retort |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 372-374
|
1st and 2nd pair-members |
|
Lecture 16 |
p. 383
|
“My” vis-à-vis Relational Pairs |
|
Lecture 21 |
pp. 419-420
|
Insult-Retort, Q-A, Commands-Returns |
|
Lecture 26 |
p. 445
|
Paired categories |
|
|
|||
W ’67 |
March 2 |
p. 528
|
Questions as “sequentially relevant” - get answers |
|
|||
F ’67 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 667-674
|
Various |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 675-676
|
The “unit character” of utterance pairs |
|
Lecture 8 |
p. 685
|
‘Expansions’ on pair sequences: pre-sequences |
|
Volume II |
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 189-190
|
“Greetings come in pairs” |
|
|||
S ’72 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 521--532
|
Adjacency pairs |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 533-537
|
Adjacency pairs |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 565
|
Pair vs. topical organization |
Paradox |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 162
|
“I decided that years ago” |
|
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 19 |
pp. 404-405
|
“I talk in my sleep” |
Lecture 21 |
pp. 422-424
|
“I decided that years ago” |
|
|
|||
F ’67 |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 693-699
|
“I decided that years ago” |
Paranoia |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 – S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 19
|
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 35
|
|
|
|
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 12 |
p. 356
|
|
Paraphrasing |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 – S ’65
|
Lecture 13 |
p. 109
|
Poems and proverbs lose power when paraphrased |
|
|||
F ’67 |
Lecture 8 Lecture 14 |
p. 687
p. 739
pp. 739-740
|
Paraphrased quotation of (combined) Q-A is appropriate version of what answerer said
“His mommy…told him to get out of the road.”
Q-A paraphrased as answerer’s ‘statement’
|
Volume II |
|||
W ’69
|
Lecture 3 |
pp. 112-113
|
Academic notion of showing understanding is to paraphrase |
Pause (See: ‘Silence / Pause’) |
Volume I |
Volume II |
Performatives |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 10 |
pp. 343-344
|
|
Lecture 19 |
p. 404 |
||
S ’67 |
Lecture 17 |
pp. 613-615 |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 13 |
pp. 737=738 |
|
Philosophy |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 – S ’65 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 24
|
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 11 |
p. 195 |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 202 |
|
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 14 |
p. 371 |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 5 |
|
‘Phoney’ |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 – S ’65 |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 69-70
|
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 08 |
pp. 329-330 |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 12 |
pp. 580-581 |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 79-80 |
|
Placement, Positioning |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65
|
Lecture 1 |
p. 6
|
“I can’t hear you” usable anywhere |
Lecture 3 |
p. 31
|
Mid-conversation occurrence or not? |
|
|
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 9 |
p. 183
|
Positioning freedom of clausal constructions |
Lecture 04.b |
pp. 296-299 |
Priority Activities
|
|
|
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 309
|
“By the way”: position marker |
Lecture 10 |
pp. 344-346
pp. 346-347
|
“I still say though…”
Positioned categories |
|
Lecture 17 |
p. 408
|
Positioning freedom of clausal constructions |
|
|
|||
W ’67 |
March 9 |
pp. 538, 542
p. 543
|
Positioned topic introduction
“Too” as a speaker-positioning marker |
|
|||
S ’67 |
Lecture 17 |
p. 614
|
“I still say though…” |
|
|||
F ’67 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 633, 634-635
|
Various |
Lecture 3 |
p. 642
|
“Interruptions” |
|
Lecture 4 |
p. 650
|
Next utterance at completion |
|
Lecture 5 |
pp. 661-662
|
Appendor Questions |
|
Lecture 7 |
pp. 681-682
|
Floor seekers |
|
Lecture 13 |
p. 733ff
|
Mis-hearings |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 745-746
|
Laughter & “Uh huh” |
|
|
|||
S ’68 |
May 8 |
p. 775
p. 777
|
Announcement of reason for a call often placed at beginning of call
‘Timing’ of a call (e.g. 4:00am) |
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 47
|
Placement and “adjacency relationship” |
|
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 87-91
|
Placement of noticings, announcements, etc. |
|
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 188-189
p. 199
|
Placement of ‘greetings’
Emotions placed in phone call openings |
|
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 247-248
|
Placement of stories in a conversation; in time…vis-à-vis story’s importance
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 254
|
“I still say though” |
|
Lecture 7 |
pp. 273-274
|
Adjacent vs. distanced relevant utterances |
|
|
|||
W ’71 |
March 11 |
p. 320
|
Placement of noticings |
|
|||
S ’71 |
April 9 |
pp. 352-353
|
“Speaker specifically place almost all of their utterances.” |
April 12 |
pp. 357-358
|
Placed utterances |
|
April 17 |
p. 364
|
Placing ‘closings’ |
|
May 10 |
pp. 393-394
|
Call to SPC placed in the course-of emotional trouble |
|
May 21 |
p. 403
|
Positioning of closings |
|
|
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 422-423
|
Story recipients place an understanding upon completion of a story. Proverbial expressions used to understand with. So, proverbial expressions commonly occur in story completions.
|
Lecture 2 |
p. 425
p. 427
|
A place for putting ‘understandings’ is directly on completion of a story
An utterance’s positioning can be a resource for finding what it’s talking to.
|
|
Lecture 6 |
p. 455
|
Assertion of “normal [temporal] positioning of sex for a date.” |
|
Lecture 9 |
p. 473
|
“Next morning” vis-à-vis “last night”, “second door” vis-à-vis “first door”
|
|
|
|||
S ’72 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 530
|
Positioning an utterance vis-à-vis adjacency pairs (incl. “I did, too”) |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 538, 539-540
|
“The oldest one in the class” as a “unique position”, like “the only cop/ Negro / woman in the class”
|
|
Lecture 4 |
pp. 554-560
pp. 557-558
|
‘Next position’
“I still say though” |
|
Lecture 5 |
pp. 567-568
|
“Anyway” |
|
Lecture 6 |
pp. 572-574
pp. 574-575
p. 575
|
Placement of expressions of sorrow and joy (“very very early on into the conversation”)
Placement of interactionally generated invitations after a ‘thankable’
“Positioning load” of an utterance; e.g. “Anyway” |
Playing Dumb, Feigning Innocence, etc. |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 – S ’65
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 83
|
|
|
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 6 |
p. 163
|
|
|
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 15 |
pp. 379-380
|
|
‘Poetics’ of Ordinary Talk |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 186-187
|
Double adjectives: e.g. “Dirty grubby”, “teeny weeny” |
Volume II |
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 264
p. 265
|
‘Vision’ conceptions for blind lady’s problem
Sound relationship ‘preoccupied’ (‘ocular’) |
|
|||
W ’71 |
February 19 |
pp. 291-293
|
“God → soul → only one” / “Oh God → gotten” semantic and sound relationships
|
March 4 |
pp. 305-309
pp. 314-315
|
“Sound sequences”
‘Latching on’ |
|
March 11 |
pp. 321-325
|
Various |
|
|
|||
S ’71 |
April 5 |
pp. 341-344
|
Sound / contrast |
May 17 |
pp. 396-401
|
Spatialized characterizations |
|
|
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 420-424 |
Pun: “Something to look up to” (vis-à-vis “roof”) |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 425-428
|
Pun: “Something to look up to” (vis-à-vis “roof”) |
|
Lecture 3 |
pp. 431-435
pp. 435-436
|
Allusive talk / punning “skirting the subject”, the subject being sex
Flurry of visual terms |
|
Lecture 15 |
p. 505
|
“We SPENT…” Here used for ‘time’, where money is an unmentioned, complainable expenditure “He GAVE no…” Here used for a ‘reason’, where gifts is a ungiven giveable.
|
Poetry |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 13 |
pp. 108-109 |
|
F ’65 |
Lecture 6 |
p. 165 |
|
S ’66 |
Lecture 20 |
p. 414 |
|
F ’67 |
Introduction |
p. 621 |
|
Lecture 4 |
pp. 650-651 |
||
Lecture 5 |
p. 664 |
||
S ’68 |
April 17 |
pp. 755-756 |
|
Volume II |
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 216, 217 |
|
Politeness |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’67 |
Lecture 10 |
p. 705 |
|
Volume II |
Possessables and Possessitives |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 16 |
pp. 383-387 |
|
S ’67 |
Lecture 16 |
pp. 605-609 |
|
Volume II |
Power |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 – S ’66 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 59 |
“Power is a zero-sum phenomenon” |
Volume II |
Practical Mysticism |
|||
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 147-148 |
|
Pre-sequences |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’66
|
Lecture 4 |
pp. 302-305
|
Pre-invitations / rejections (1st speaker) |
|
|||
F ’67 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 685-691
|
Various |
|
|||
S ’68 |
April 24 |
pp. 765-766
|
Story ‘pre-beginnings’ |
May 8 |
p. 773
|
Pre-acceptance or rejection (asking for a reason for a call) |
|
Volume II |
|||
S ’72 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 529
|
“Pre-signalling” e.g., invitations |
Preference |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’66
|
Lecture 8 |
p. 337
|
A “preferred category” for identifying someone |
Lecture 13 |
p. 367
|
[X] “is not a matter of theoretical preference for the players” |
|
|
|||
W ’67 |
March 9 |
p. 540
|
A specific conversational “move” is “not legitimate, conventional, preferred, etc.”
|
|
|||
F ’67 |
Lecture 12 |
p. 725
|
Re. obscene puns: “on the one hand, routinely one hearing is so preferred that the other is not even considered or heard, and on the other,… in special circumstances the usual preferred hearing is rejected.”
|
Volume II | |||
W ’69 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 96-97
p. 97
|
Local explanations, for whatever, are preferred explanations, if they can be used.”
Preference for “local expenses” |
Lecture 8 |
p. 133
|
Preference for “concrete conversation”? |
|
Lecture 9 |
p. 144
pp. 147-149
|
Preference for ‘if-then’ multi-clause sentences over clause + ‘and’, ‘but’, etc.
Preference for description that the other knows |
|
|
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 207
|
Preference for offers over requests |
|
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 223-224
|
Preference for current selects next, over next self-selects |
|
|||
S ’71 |
April 23 |
pp. 367-368
|
‘First preference invitations’: “If the invitation is not for the first preference, then you’re indicating that a first preference is not present.” (‘dinner' vs. e.g. ‘talk’)
|
April 26 |
p. 374
|
“ ‘Talk’ as a non-preferred event replaceable with some preferred event, ‘get drunk’. Preferred characterizations of Saturday evening.”
|
|
May 21 |
p. 409
|
Preference for going second |
|
May 24 |
p. 413
pp. 414-415
|
‘Not hearing’ “preferably said” over ‘not understanding’
‘Questioner-preferred answers’ |
|
|
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 444-452
p. 456
|
Preference for Type I over Type II identifications
Abstract sense of ‘preference’: as a first alternative. (Louise's sex story) |
Lecture 13 |
p. 496
|
Current speaker selects next as “preferred” |
|
|
|||
S ’72 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 524
|
“Ordering of preferences” vis-à-vis next speaker selection |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 565-566
pp. 566-567
|
A question can “prefer something an answer long or more than an answer long (the letter being e.g., “a ‘topic opener’”.)
“…A preference for…using aspects of the [conversation’s opening] to get directly into topic talk…”
|
|
Lecture 6 |
p. 572
|
Expressions of sorrow and joy related “preferentially” to each other: sorrow first or only
|
Preservable Features of an Interaction for Another Interaction |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’68 |
May 8 |
p. 773 |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 49 |
Complaints |
|
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 165 |
“The first thing he said was…” |
|
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 8 |
p. 283 |
A question asked / answered in a news interview, quoted on a TV show, quoted by a TV-viewer to his friends |
|
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 458-465 |
ADATO Insurance salesman story: retold after a long delay |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 466-469 |
(ditto) |
Private Calendars |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 – S ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 36-38 |
|
Volume II |
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 167 |
|
|
|||
S ’72 |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 564-565 |
Public disasters as “calendrical” vis-à-vis ‘the last time we talked’ |
Programmatic Relevance |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 – S ’65 |
Lecture 5 |
p. 39 |
|
Volume II |
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 336-340 |
Of knowledge formulated via MCD categories |
Appendix A |
pp. 493-495 |
In children’s games |
|
|
|||
S ’67 |
Lecture 12 |
p. 578 |
Of categorical, ‘Y do X’ statements |
Lecture 13 |
p. 588 |
Kids learn it early |
Pronouns (See ‘Indexical Expressions’) |
Volume I |
Volume II |
Proverbs |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 – S ’65 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 24 |
|
Lecture 8 |
p. 62 |
And ‘normal events’ |
|
Lecture 10 |
p. 78 |
And kids’ case-by case Class I, II inquiries |
|
Lecture 13 |
pp. 104-110 |
||
F ’65 |
Lecture 6 |
pp. 166, 167 |
‘Proverbially correct’ |
S ’67 |
Lecture 13 |
pp. 587-588 |
Tautological proverbs |
Volume II |
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 422-424 |
Used to show understanding |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 426-430 |
“Ideal objects to do ‘understanding’ with” |
Psychiatric Issues, Materials, etc. |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64 – S ’65 |
Lecture 1
Lecture 2 |
p. 3
p. 10
p. 19 |
|
Lecture 4 |
p. 28
p. 29 |
|
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 33 p. 35 |
|
|
Lecture 6 |
pp. 46-47 |
|
|
Lecture 8 |
p. 60 |
|
|
Lecture 10 |
pp. 76-80 |
|
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 93 |
|
|
Lecture 13 |
pp. 109-110 |
|
|
Lecture 14 |
p. 113ff |
|
|
|
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 12 |
pp. 199-203 |
|
Lecture 14 |
p. 214 |
|
|
|
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 4 |
pp. 302-304 |
|
Lecture 7 |
pp. 324-325 |
|
|
Lecture 10 |
p. 347 |
|
|
Lecture 12 |
pp. 354-356 |
|
|
Lecture 13 |
pp. 364-365 |
|
|
Lecture 18 |
p. 398
pp. 400-401 |
|
|
Lecture 19 |
p. 406 |
|
|
Lecture 20 |
p. 411
p. 413 |
|
|
Lecture 23 |
p. 433 |
|
|
Lecture 27 |
pp. 452-454 |
|
|
Lecture 28 |
p. 459 |
|
|
|
|||
F ’67 |
Lecture 14 |
p. 742 |
|
|
|||
S ’68 |
April 24 |
p. 768 |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 28 |
|
|
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 3 |
pp. 105-106 |
|
Lecture 8 |
pp. 135-136 |
|
|
|
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 217 |
|
Lecture 5 |
pp. 259-260 |
|
|
|
|||
W ’71 |
March 4 |
p. 312 |
|
|
|||
S ’71 |
April 30 |
pp. 376-383 |
SPC: NYE |
May 3 |
pp. 384-390 |
SPC: NYE |
|
May 10 |
pp. 391-395 |
SPC: NYE |
|
May 17 |
pp. 396-401 |
SPC: NYE |
|
May 21 |
pp. 402-409 |
SPC: NYE |
|
|
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 426 |
|
Lecture 16 |
p. 514 |
|
Puns |
|||
Volume I |
|||
S ’66 |
Lecture 27 |
p. 451 |
|
F ’67 |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 699-700 |
(As emerging from “routine” productions) |
Lecture 12 |
p. 725 |
Obscene puns |
Questions, Answers, Q-A |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 4, 8
|
|
Lecture 4 |
p. 29
|
|
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 32
|
|
|
Lecture 6 |
p. 40
|
|
|
Lecture 7 |
pp. 49-56
pp. 49, 51-52
p. 56
|
‘Chaining rule’ (not by that name)
Answer constructed re. project of questions (cf. p. 688) |
|
Lecture 12 |
p. 96
p. 102
p. 103
|
‘The ‘chain’ possibility” |
|
|
|||
F ’65 |
Lecture 3 |
p. 144
|
The 10 ‘initiation ceremony’ “questions” |
Lecture 4 |
p. 156
|
|
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 158
|
|
|
Lecture 8 |
pp. 176-177
|
|
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 211-212, 217-219
|
|
|
Appendix B |
p. 231
|
“What are you doing?” “Nothing” |
|
|
|||
S ’66
|
Lecture 2 |
p. 256
pp. 256-257
|
Qs get As; “‘the chaining rule’.”
“You know what?” “What?” |
Lecture 2 (R) |
p. 264
|
Qs get As; “‘the chaining rule’.” |
|
Lecture 04.a |
p. 264
|
Non-Q-intend recognizable Qs |
|
Lecture 5 |
pp. 309-311
|
|
|
Lecture 7 |
p. 324
|
Q-Q-A-A (cf. p. 55) |
|
Lecture 11 |
p. 350
|
Answers with “built-in defenses” |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 373-374
p. 373
|
As 1st and 2nd pair-members
Re. Q-intonations [“What’s my Line” data] |
|
Lecture 15 |
pp. 380-381
|
Correction-invitation device (“No” + a 'good' reason) |
|
Lecture 27 |
p. 453
|
Questions which give information about askers |
|
|
|||
S ’67 |
Lecture 9 |
pp. 564-565
|
Lying answers to which-type questions |
|
|||
F ’67 |
Introduction |
pp. 619-621
|
“What are you going to do?” |
Lecture 5 |
pp. 659-664
|
Appendor questions |
|
Lecture 6 |
pp. 667-674
|
(As ‘paired utterances’) |
|
Lecture 8 |
p. 687
p. 688
|
Paraphrased quotations of Q-A as statements
Answers directed to the sequence of which the question is seen to be a part (cf. p. 56)
|
|
Lecture 9 |
p. 696
|
“What use would it have?” (none) |
|
Lecture 14 |
pp. 740-743
|
In the fit between Qs and As |
|
Volume II |
|||
F ’68 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 25
|
‘Answers’: 2nds; non-sentences |
Lecture 4 |
p. 47
|
Placement of possible answer of the question |
|
|
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 7 |
pp. 124-125
|
Answer / Comment-as-Answer relationship vis-à-vis Q-Q-Q etc. “Why?” “Why not?” “What do you mean ‘why not’?” |
|
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 2 |
pp. 176-178
|
A question which conveys information requires only a “yes/no” answer to the question, no response to the conveyed information
|
|
|||
S ’70 |
Lecture 8 |
pp. 282-284
|
On asking questions |
|
|||
S ’71 |
April 5 |
pp. 344-346
|
Q-q-a-A; strategies for avoiding going 1st |
April 30 |
pp. 380-383
|
‘Face sheet’ questions |
|
May 24 |
pp. 412-415
|
“Yes” vs. “No”- plus or “Yes but” |
|
|
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 2 |
p. 427
|
“Yes” / “No” answer vis-à-vis positioning and understanding |
|
|||
S ’72 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 528, 529
p. 530
|
Q [q-a] A: “insertion sequences”
Grammatical non-sentence as adequate complete 2nd pair parts, e.g. Answers
|
Lecture 2 |
p. 535
pp. 537-539
|
Repeated Q’s “do not follow a 1st pair part, they follow the pause after a 1st pair part.”
Q-A: “Are you the oldest one in the class?” “Oh, by far.” |
|
Lecture 3 |
pp. 547-548
|
To the Q “Who is calling?” there are alternative types of answers |
|
Lecture 4 |
p. 559
|
One-word questions (e.g. “Why?”) and ‘appendor questions’ (e.g. “Until when?”)
|
|
Lecture 5 |
pp. 561-569
|
Q-A: “How did you survive…?” (as a “How have you been?”) |
Reason-for-a-Call |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 10 |
pp. 73-74 |
Calls as ‘accountable actions’ vs. “No reason” |
|
|||
S ’68 |
May 8 |
pp. 773-779 |
|
May 29 |
p. 793 |
|
|
Volume II |
|||
W ’69 |
Lecture 1 |
p. 88 |
|
|
|||
W ’70 |
Lecture 1 |
pp. 163-166 |
|
Lecture 5 |
p. 210 |
|
|
|
|||
F ’71 |
Lecture 4 |
p. 440 |
|
Recasting a Prior Action |
|||
Volume I |
|||
F ’64- S ’65 |
Lecture 2 |